Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
18 January 2017 A bare-part ornament is a stronger predictor of dominance than plumage ornamentation in the cooperatively breeding Australian Swamphen
Cody J. Dey, James S. Quinn, Ash King, Jessica Hiscox, James Dale
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Many animals use coloration to signal dominance and fighting ability. In birds, plumage coloration is often linked to individual quality, but less research has investigated coloration in unfeathered traits (i.e. ‘bare parts'), despite theoretical expectations that they might be more reliable signals. Here, we investigate the relationship between multiple ornaments and social dominance in a wild population of the cooperatively breeding Australian Swamphen (known locally as and hereafter referred to as Pūkeko; Porphyrio melanotus melanotus) to test the hypothesis that bare-part ornaments should be more strongly correlated with dominance than plumage coloration. We show that the size and color of the pūkeko's frontal shield (a bare-part ornament), as well as the brightness and chroma of the blue-UV breast plumage, are correlated with social dominance in adult birds. However, the correlation between frontal shield size and dominance was much higher than that between plumage color and dominance, and was also higher than most previously published correlations between plumage traits and dominance. Additionally, frontal shield size, but not breast coloration, was correlated with the size of the testes in male pūkekos, which may be because bare-part ornaments are more closely tied to an individual's current physiology than plumage ornaments. While correlative, our results demonstrate that bare-part and plumage traits could act as redundant ornaments, but with differing reliability, and suggest that future studies on bare-part ornaments will enhance our understanding of dominance signaling.

INTRODUCTION

Many animals use signals to communicate information about their quality to conspecifics. When signals convey information about fighting ability (also known as resource holding potential; Parker 1974), they can be used to form and maintain dominance relationships, and may therefore influence the distribution of resources among individuals (e.g., Rohwer 1975). Such signals are often referred to as ‘status signals,' and are thought to be most beneficial for species that frequently interact with unfamiliar individuals (Rohwer 1982), or species for which the costs of having ambiguous dominance relationships are high (West-Eberhard 1983).

In birds, plumage coloration often has a signaling role (Dale 2006, Hill and McGraw 2006). While most research has focused on the role of coloration in mate attraction (Hill 2006), a large body of literature suggests that coloration is also used as a status signal (Senar 2006, Tibbetts and Safran 2009, Dey et al. 2015). In this context, numerous studies of model signal traits (e.g., the chest badge of the Harris's Sparrow [Zonotrichia querula]) have been invaluable toward determining the specific manner in which avian status signals function. However, many birds have complex patterns of coloration and researchers have become increasingly interested in exploring the relationships among multiple ornaments, as well as investigating the specific information conveyed by different types of coloration in a social context (Møller and Pomiankowski 1993, Chaine et al. 2011). Indeed, studying single ornamental traits may provide misleading results if receivers incorporate multiple ornaments into an integrated signal (Hegyi et al. 2015) or if they consider the congruence among multiple ornaments during assessment of a conspecific (Tibbetts and Izzo 2010). As a result, several recent studies have examined the relationship between different plumage ornaments within the same species (Chaine and Lyon 2008, Chaine et al. 2011, Laucht and Dale 2012, Mercadante and Hill 2014, Hegyi et al. 2015). In general, these studies have found that multiple ornaments may provide a varying amount of overlap in information content (Laucht and Dale 2012), with some sets of ornaments showing high covariation and functioning as a single signal, and other sets signaling different aspects of quality that are unrelated to one another.

In contrast to plumage ornamentation, less research attention has been devoted to ornamentation in avian bare parts (e.g., legs, bills, and wattles). Like plumage, bare-part ornamentation is used in both mate choice (Omland 1996, Torres and Velando 2003) and rival assessment (Shawcross and Slater 1984, Ligon et al. 1990, Bright and Waas 2002, Murphy et al. 2009). However, bare-part traits are considered to be dynamic traits because they are able to change size or color over short time periods (i.e. hours or days; Faivre et al. 2003, Ardia et al. 2010), whereas change in plumage ornamentation is generally restricted to discrete molting periods. As a result, it has been suggested that bare-part traits may be better suited to signaling in aggressive contexts than plumage traits, as they may provide more accurate information about an individual's current condition and/or aggressive intent (Dey et al. 2015). Indeed, bare-part ornaments are correlated with social dominance in several species (e.g., Moss et al. 1979, Buchholz 1995, Emlen and Wrege 2004), yet few studies have directly compared plumage and bare-part ornamentation to evaluate the information content and reliability of these different signal modalities.

Androgens mediate dominance behavior in many animals (e.g., Joslyn 1973, Taves et al. 2009) and, since many ornaments are also androgen-dependent, these hormones are thought to be a key aspect of individual condition that is linked with social interactions and signal phenotype in vertebrates (Tibbetts 2014, Vitousek et al. 2014). In male birds, androgen levels are often correlated with the size of the testes, and there have been several studies demonstrating a relationship between the size of the testes and plumage ornamentation (e.g., Kimball and Ligon 1999, Merilä and Sheldon 1999). However, the correlation between these factors is typically low, perhaps because plumage ornamentation is determined by physiological conditions during molt, while the testes can change in size with environmental and social conditions. Most birds also have asymmetrical testes (Briskie and Montgomerie 2007, Calhim and Montgomerie 2015), and variation in testicular asymmetry has been argued to be related to individual condition, with higher-quality individuals having more asymmetrical testes (Møller 1994, Calhim and Birkhead 2009). Indeed, Møller (1994) demonstrated that testicular asymmetry was positively related to plumage ornamentation in Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) and House Sparrows (Passer domesticus). However, bare-part ornamentation may be more strongly correlated with testicular investment as many bare-part traits are androgen-dependent (Moss et al. 1979, Ligon et al. 1990, Eens et al. 2000, Ardia et al. 2010).

In this study, we compared bare-part and plumage-based ornamentation in the Australian Swamphen (Porphyrio melanotus melanotus; known locally and referred to hereafter as Pūkeko). In this cooperatively breeding bird, most individuals live in polygynandrous groups that defend a shared territory and cooperate to raise the group's offspring (Craig 1980). Pūkeko social structure shows considerable interpopulation variation (e.g., Jamieson 1997), but at our study site groups typically contain 5–15 individuals and had approximately equal sex ratios (C. Dey personal observation). Within social groups, Pūkeko form mixed-sex dominance hierarchies (Dey and Quinn 2014), which influence access to reproductive opportunities (Craig 1980, Jamieson and Craig 1987) and nonsexual resources (Craig 1977). Additionally, when there are multiple breeding females in a group, they all lay in a single nest (Craig 1980). This phenomenon is known as ‘joint laying,' and can lead to intragroup conflict because hatching success decreases with increasing total clutch size (Quinn et al. 2012, Dey et al. 2014b).

Like many other rails (e.g., Gullion 1951, Petrie 1988, Crowley and Magrath 2004), Pūkeko of both sexes have a fleshy frontal shield ornament that extends from the bill onto the crown. In Pūkeko, this ornament is shaped like a guitar pick, with the widest part of the shield located distally from the bill, and changes from black to bright red once birds reach sexual maturity. In other rails (e.g., American Coots [Fulica Americana]), the shield is highly vascularized, ranges from 2.0 mm to 3.5 mm thick, and is covered in a pigmented, keratinized callus layer (Gullion 1951); however, similar histological data is not available for Pūkeko. Regardless, previous studies have shown that the size of the Pūkeko's frontal shield is correlated with social dominance (Dey and Quinn 2014), and a recent experiment demonstrated that frontal shields are used as status signals (Dey et al. 2014a). Despite several studies on frontal shield size in Pūkeko, there have been no examinations of variation in frontal shield color. Furthermore, Pūkeko also have brilliant blue–violet ventral plumage that is displayed during aggressive interactions (C. Dey personal observation). Whether this ventral plumage has a signaling function is currently unknown. As a first step toward understanding multiple ornamentation in Pūkeko, we performed multivariate analyses of the relationships between ornament characteristics and key life history (i.e. sex, body size), environmental (season), and physiological traits (size and asymmetry of the testes). Additionally, we examined relationships between ornaments and measures of social dominance to test the general prediction that bare-part ornamentation should be more strongly correlated with dominance than plumage ornamentation. While our study was correlative, the breadth of our data allowed us to simultaneously consider, and control for, a variety of factors that may influence Pūkeko ornamentation, but which have often been neglected in other studies.

METHODS

This study was conducted at the Tawharanui Open Sanctuary (36°22′S, 174°49′E) on the North Island of New Zealand. At this site, Pūkeko live in permanent, mixed-sex social groups of 5–15 individuals that defend an all-purpose territory. Further details on Pūkeko social organization and reproductive ecology are published elsewhere (Jamieson et al. 1994, Jamieson 1997, Dey et al. 2012, Quinn et al. 2012). From 2008 to 2013, we captured adult Pūkeko during 5 sampling periods (see Table 1) using trapping procedures that are described elsewhere (Quinn et al. 2012). At the time of first capture, each individual was fitted with a numbered metal band and a unique combination of 4 colored plastic bands to enable us to identify individuals through time. We collected several morphological measurements, including frontal shield width, which is highly correlated with the overall area of the frontal shield ornament (R2 = 0.87, n = 50). We sexed birds by measurement (Craig et al. 1980) and collected breast plumage and frontal shield coloration data (see below).

TABLE 1.

Sampling periods and the number of captured Australian Swamphen, or Pūkeko (Porphyrio melanotus melanotus), used in this study.

i0004-8038-134-2-317-t01.tif

a

Birds were collected postmortem following a partial cull of the study population.

In May and June of 2014, 108 previously banded individuals were collected postmortem immediately after a partial cull of the population (performed by the landowner under New Zealand Department of Conservation Wildlife Authority Permit #38671-FAU). We measured the frontal shield width and breast plumage coloration of these birds. In addition, for the 60 male birds in the sample, the right and left testis were separated and weighed to the nearest 0.01 g with a digital scale.

In total, measurements from 469 captures of 272 individuals were considered in this study (Table 1). Not all types of data were collected during each capture (see below) and therefore sample sizes varied among analyses.

Frontal Shield Coloration

During sampling periods 2–4 (Table 1), we took standardized digital photographs of Pūkeko frontal shields to quantify shield coloration. For each captured individual, we took 3 photographs using a Fujifilm Finepix S700 camera (Fujifilm, Tokyo, Japan). To standardize photography conditions, the photographs were taken indoors, from a set distance (50 cm), and were illuminated primarily by the camera's flash. The subject was oriented such that the transverse plane of the frontal shield was held parallel to the lens of the camera (i.e. the full surface area of the frontal shield was visible), and the same individual (C. Dey) held the bird in all photographs. To control for variation between pictures, we also included a red color standard within each picture.

Frontal shield coloration was quantified using the colorZapper package (Valcu and Dale 2014) in R (R Core Team 2015). For each photograph, we selected a polygon that encompassed the typical color of the individual's frontal shield and avoided any areas of glare caused by the camera's flash. The colorZapper package then calculated the mean intensity of red (R), green (G), and blue (B) coloration (on a scale of 0 to 255) for 400 randomly chosen pixels within the polygon. Although this method does not consider UV reflectance, spectrophotometry has shown that Pūkeko frontal shields do not have substantial reflectance in the UV range (see electronic supplementary material in Dey et al. 2014a). RGB values were then used in a principal component (PC) analysis (Table 2), with PC1 being related to total reflectance (R2 (R + G + B) = 0.95) and PC2 being a measure of redness. Preliminary analysis suggested that measured shield brightness was highly variable among photos (intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) measured from 7 individuals captured on 2 separate occasions within a 1 week period = 0.38) and was not related to behavioral or life history variables; thus, we only considered PC2 for all further analyses (ICC = 0.60). Although measurement repeatability of PC2 was high, PC2 values were also correlated with the measured ‘value' (part of the hue–saturation–value [HSV] color space) of the red color standard (R2 = 0.26, n = 130), suggesting that some PC2 variation among individuals was attributable to variation in photographic conditions. As a result, we ran a regression of PC2 against the ‘value' of the red color standard, and used the residuals as our shield color score in subsequent analyses.

TABLE 2.

Principal component loadings of Pūkeko frontal shield coloration measured in red–green–blue (RGB) color space.

i0004-8038-134-2-317-t02.tif

Breast Plumage Coloration

To quantify Pūkeko breast coloration, we collected 8–10 semiplume feathers from the breast of each adult captured during sampling periods 1, 2, 3, 4, and 6 (Table 1). Feathers were cut from the center of the breast, directly over the sternal keel, and were stored in plastic bags for later analysis. Although we did not directly test whether this method produced similar results to measuring plumage coloration on live birds, similar methods performed on Eurasian Great Tits (Parus major), which have much smaller feathers than Pūkeko, showed that measurements taken from 10–15 feathers produced similar results to directly measuring live birds (Quesada and Senar 2006). We collected UV–Vis reflectance values by stacking the feathers atop a black card and taking spectral readings from the distal, pennaceous portion of the feather, which is the only part of the feather that contains the blue–violet coloration. We used an Ocean Optics (Dunedin, Florida, USA) USB2000 spectrophotometer with a PX-2 pulsed xenon light source to collect reflectance for wavelengths between 300 and 700 nm (integration time: 100 ms; 5 readings averaged per recording; boxcar 3). At the end of the combined reflection probe and light source, there was a nonreflective black pointer cut at a 45° angle. Two readings were taken from each feather stack (one from each side of the rachis, with the probe held parallel to the feather barbs). Then, we used the spectrograms to calculate mean brightness and chroma for each reading (mean brightness = average reflectance between 300 and 700 nm; chroma = [maximum reflectance − minimum reflectance]/mean brightness; Montgomerie 2006). We averaged the values from each side of the rachis to produce one value for chroma and brightness for each sample. Although plumage hue (i.e. peak wavelength) is a common color measurement that has been linked to quality in a number of studies, our measurement of hue had low repeatability (ICC = 0.01), which may have been due in part to overall low intraspecific variability and/or iridescence in Pūkeko breast plumage. As a result, here we consider only breast plumage chroma and mean brightness, which were highly repeatable for samples measured on 2 separate occasions (ICC = 0.73 and 0.63, respectively; n = 10).

Dominance Observations

To determine the relationship between ornamentation and social dominance, in January and February, 2012, we performed behavioral observations of 11 social groups in which all adult group members were distinguishable by band combinations. Details of the observation methodology are published elsewhere (Dey and Quinn 2014, Dey et al. 2014a). Briefly, we performed 10 30-min behavioral observations of each social group and recorded all dominance interactions (i.e. subordinate postures, displacements, pecks, kicks, and charges; Craig 1977) that occurred among group members (average number of interactions during 5 hr of observation = 85, range = 5–241). All interactions were pooled and used to calculate David's dominance score (David 1987) for each individual in each of the 11 social groups. This method is appropriate for dominance ranking when not all pairs of individuals interact (David 1987), and incorporates information about both the number of opponents that are dominated, as well as the rank of those opponents. Blinded observations (e.g., of individual ID or ornamentation) were not possible, as we were observing focal animals in the field. However, observers did not have access to morphometric or colorimetric data for the observed birds, and variation in ornamentation was virtually indistinguishable from the distance at which observations were performed.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed with R statistical software (R Core Team 2015), using the lme4 (Bates et al. 2015), lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al. 2015), and MuMIn (Bartoń 2015) packages. The goals of our analyses were: (1) to determine whether each type of ornamentation (shield size, shield color, breast plumage brightness, and breast plumage chroma) was correlated with social dominance, and whether each ornament was correlated with dominance after controlling for potentially important confounding variables, (2) to determine which type of ornamentation was the best predictor of dominance, and (3) to test whether male ornamentation was related to gonadal investment. In all analyses, we scaled continuous predictor variables by 2× their standard deviation to make the regression coefficients comparable with binary predictor variables (Gelman 2008).

First, we ran 4 separate linear mixed effects (LME) models to test for a relationship between ornamentation and David's dominance score. In each model, the dominance score was included as the response variable, and the ornamentation score was included as a fixed effect. When multiple ornamentation scores were available for an individual, we used the measurements taken in closest temporal proximity to the behavioral observations. In each model, we also included social group ID as a random intercept to account for shared variance among individuals living in the same social group. Including this random effect improved model fit for all 4 models.

Although the models described above were a direct test of the relationships between dominance and ornamentation, the sample size for some models was small because not all types of ornamentation were measured for each individual (see Tables 3 and 4) and direct dominance observations were only performed on a subset of individuals. As a result, we ran a second set of models using body mass as a proxy for dominance, which allowed us to use a much larger number of samples and to control for important life history variables that could have influenced the relationship between dominance and ornamentation. In Pūkeko, body mass is strongly correlated with dominance score (marginal R2 = 0.50; Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2013). We used log-transformed body mass as the response variable in our models, and included ornamentation score, sex, and season (a binary score of whether or not measurements were taken during the peak breeding season of August–November; Dey and Jamieson 2013) as fixed effects. In each model, year and bird ID were included as random intercepts to account for shared variance among individuals captured in the same year and for repeated measures of individuals. Importantly, body mass is not intrinsically linked to frontal shield size (or any other ornamentation) in Pūkeko; experimental studies have shown that changes in shield size occur without changes in body mass (Dey et al. 2014a), and that shield size is significantly correlated with dominance even after controlling for body mass (Dey et al. 2014a). While we did not have age data for most individuals in this study (and therefore could not include age in our models), individuals who were recaptured in different sampling periods did not show changes in average ornamentation score between captures (C. Dey personal observation), suggesting that there is no consistent increase or decrease in ornamentation with age after Pūkeko reach adulthood.

TABLE 3.

Results of linear mixed modeling for Pūkeko shield ornamentation. For categorical predictor variables, the level of the variable that corresponds with the effect is indicated in brackets (e.g., Sex [male] is the effect for the difference between ‘male' and the reference level).

i0004-8038-134-2-317-t03.tif

TABLE 4.

Results of linear mixed modeling for Pūkeko breast plumage coloration. For categorical predictor variables, the level of the variable that corresponds with the effect is indicated in brackets (e.g. Sex [male] is the effect for the difference between ‘male' and the reference level).

i0004-8038-134-2-317-t04.tif

Because different ornaments were measured on different individuals, we could not perform statistical analyses that included all ornaments in the same model. Instead, we used meta-analytic techniques to determine which ornament best predicted social dominance. To perform this analysis, we ran a series of LME models, and calculated correlation coefficients (r) from t-statistics using methods outlined in Nakagawa and Cuthill (2007). In the first 4 models, dominance score was used as the response variable, with shield size, shield color, breast chroma, or breast brightness the sole fixed effect in each of the 4 models. In these models, social group ID was again included as a random intercept. We also ran 4 additional models using log-transformed body mass, a strong proxy for social dominance, as the response variable. These models had much larger sample sizes than those that used observed dominance scores, and therefore allowed for more confidence in the fitted relationship between ornamentation and dominance. In these 4 models, the 4 ornamentation scores (described above) were again used as sole fixed effects (1 in each model). We also included year and bird ID as random intercepts to account for shared variance among observations from the same year and repeated measures of some individuals. To analyze significant differences among types of ornamentation, we bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for regression coefficients using the parametric bootstrap function bootMer in the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2015). We considered 2 ornaments to have significantly different correlations with dominance if their confidence intervals did not overlap, which is a conservative test for rejection of the null hypothesis at α = 0.05 (Schenker and Gentleman 2001).

To analyze the relationship between ornamentation and testicular investment, we ran a set of linear models (LM) using measurements from males that were collected immediately following a cull in 2014 (sampling period 6). Importantly, the birds used for this analysis were sampled outside the breeding season, and therefore interindividual variation in the size of the testes was unlikely to have been due to variation in reproductive condition, but instead was likely reflective of differences in individual quality (e.g., androgen levels). In these models, we again used ornamentation as the response variable, and used the size of the testes [log(right testis mass + left testis mass)] and asymmetry of the testes [log(left testis mass/right testis mass)] as the predictor variables. Shield color was not measured for these birds, so the analysis was performed on frontal shield size and breast plumage coloration only.

RESULTS

Ornamentation and Social Dominance

All types of ornamentation considered in this study (shield size, shield color, breast chroma, and breast brightness) showed a significant positive relationship with social dominance (Tables 3 and 4, Figures 1 and 2). Additionally, frontal shield size and breast plumage brightness were significantly related to body mass, a proxy for social dominance (Tables 3 and 4), even after controlling for sex and season. As expected, males had greater body mass than females. Additionally, Pūkeko were slightly less heavy in the nonbreeding season in some models.

FIGURE 1.

The relationship between social dominance and 4 aspects of ornamentation in the Australian Swamphen, or Pūkeko (Porphyrio melanotus melanotus). Samples from male birds are shown with circles, while samples from female birds are shown with squares. Dark gray shading is created by overlapping data points. Dominance score was determined from direct observations of Pūkeko social groups and calculated using the method of David (1987). All aspects of ornamentation had a significant relationship with dominance in linear mixed-effects models (see Tables 3 and 4). Figure created using ggplot2 (Wickham 2009).

i0004-8038-134-2-317-f01.tif

FIGURE 2.

Reflectance spectra for Pūkeko breast plumage. Shown are the 95% confidence intervals of the mean reflectance for Pūkeko in relation to various morphological, behavioral, and life history variables: (A) sex, (B) shield width, (C) body mass, (D) dominance score, (E) season, and (F) testes mass. For plotting purposes only, continuous predictor variables are shown as binary factors based on whether they were above or below thresholds shown in the panels. See Table 4 for results of statistical analysis.

i0004-8038-134-2-317-f02.tif

Which Ornament Best Predicts Dominance?

Frontal shield width had the highest correlation with social dominance (Figure 3) when considering both direct (r = 0.65, 95% CL = 0.54, 0.76) and indirect dominance measures (r = 0.84, 95% CL = 0.77, 0.92). However, confidence intervals for the relationships between direct dominance scores and ornamentation were broad and therefore the confidence intervals among all ornaments overlapped (Figure 3). Using body mass, an indirect measure of social dominance with a much larger sample size, the confidence interval for shield width did not overlap with that of other ornaments (Figure 3), and this correlation was therefore significantly greater than for other forms of ornamentation.

FIGURE 3.

Correlation between ornamentation and social dominance in Pūkeko. Shown are the estimated correlation coefficients (with 95% confidence intervals) for the relationship between each ornament and either the directly observed dominance score (gray squares), or an indirect measure of dominance, body mass (black circles). Figure created using ggplot2 (Wickham 2009).

i0004-8038-134-2-317-f03.tif

Ornamentation and Testicular Investment

Consistent with most other birds, testicular asymmetry in Pūkeko was strongly left-biased (paired t-test, n = 60: mean difference in log(mass) = 0.27, 95% CL = 0.21, 0.32, P < 0.001). While the mass of the testes strongly predicted frontal shield size in male Pūkeko (LM, n = 60: estimate = 2.91, 95% CL = 2.00, 3.82, P < 0.001; Figure 4), there was no relationship between testicular asymmetry and shield size (estimate = 0.10, 95% CL = −0.81, 1.00, P = 0.83). Breast plumage chroma showed a weak and nonsignificant relationship with the mass of the testes (LM, n = 60: estimate = 0.16, 95% CL = −0.01, 0.33, P = 0.05) and was not predicted by testicular asymmetry (estimate = −0.03, 95% CL = −0.19, 0.13, P = 0.72). Additionally, neither testes mass nor asymmetry was related to breast plumage brightness (LM, n = 60: size estimate = −0.56, 95% CL = −1.40, 0.28, P = 0.19; asymmetry estimate = −0.17, 95% CL = −1.00, 0.67, P = 0.69).

FIGURE 4.

Frontal shield size is positively related to the size of the testes in Pūkeko. Shown is the relationship between frontal shield width and the size of the testes [log(left testis length + right testis length)] for 60 male Pūkeko. Figure created using ggplot2 (Wickham 2009).

i0004-8038-134-2-317-f04.tif

DISCUSSION

In this study, we have shown that frontal shield size and color, as well as breast plumage brightness and chroma, are significantly related to social dominance in the Pūkeko. However, breast plumage coloration and frontal shield coloration are weak predictors of dominance, especially after controlling for confounding variables. In contrast, frontal shield size is strongly related to dominance, and is also correlated with the size of the testes. These results are consistent with previous studies, which have suggested that frontal shield size is the primary status signal used by this species (Craig 1977, Dey et al. 2012, 2014a, Dey and Quinn 2014), but also suggest that breast plumage and frontal shield coloration may provide some additional information about individual dominance status.

Interestingly, the correlations between frontal shield size and both indices of dominance used in this study were higher than the average correlation between plumage ornaments and dominance presented in a recent meta-analysis (r = 0.359; table 2 in Santos et al. 2011). While Santos et al. (2011) showed that the relationship between plumage ornamentation and dominance is dependent on various factors (e.g., type of coloration, study design), 89% of the effect sizes considered in their meta-analysis were smaller than the correlation between shield size (a bare-part ornament) and dominance found in the current study. Conversely, the correlation between Pūkeko breast plumage coloration and dominance is consistent with the average correlation between structural plumage traits and dominance (Santos et al. 2011). This comparison generally supports the hypothesis that bare-part ornaments are more reliable predictors of dominance status than are plumage ornaments (Dey et al. 2015, this study), and suggests that an increased focus on bare-part signaling could generate new insights into the field of animal communication.

Although bare-part and plumage traits are both known to have signaling roles in birds, there are few avian species for which both of these types of ornamentation have been studied concurrently, and as such their relative importance remains unclear. Of the studies that have simultaneously considered both bare-part and plumage ornamentation, many have shown support for the ‘redundant messages' hypothesis (Møller and Pomiankowski 1993) by demonstrating that both signal modalities are correlated with aspects of quality (e.g., Jawor et al. 2004, Laucht and Dale 2012), with success in mate-choice competitions (e.g., Johnson et al. 1993, Jawor et al. 2003) or in dominance interactions (Mateos and Carranza 1997, Ramos-Fernández et al. 2004). However, other studies have demonstrated that one ornament type is more important for signaling; for example, a meta-analysis of the well-studied Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus) showed that comb characteristics, but not plumage ornamentation, were the subject of mate choice (Parker and Ligon 2003). Similarly, bill color, but not plumage ornamentation, was related to pairing success in Mallards (Anas platyrhynchos; Omland 1996). Because these studies have provided equivocal results, a synthetic, quantitative analysis (e.g., a meta-analysis) comparing the strength of the relationships between bare-part and plumage ornaments with the qualities that they might signal (i.e. dominance, attractiveness) would be valuable for the study of avian communication, and could help to guide future research.

In our study, we found a strong relationship between the size of the testes and frontal shield size in male Pūkeko. This finding is consistent with the results of studies of other species, which have demonstrated links between frontal shield characteristics and aspects of individual quality (Fenoglio et al. 2002, Alvarez et al. 2005). The size of the testes is correlated with circulating testosterone levels in many birds (Garamszegi et al. 2005), and therefore our results are consistent with a relationship between androgens and frontal shield size in Pūkeko. Importantly, our measurements of testicular size were taken outside the breeding season, suggesting that dominant Pūkeko maintain relatively high levels of circulating androgens throughout the year. Although we were not able to directly or indirectly measure androgens in females, it is likely that female shield size is also androgen-dependent. Baseline testosterone levels are correlated with bill color in female American Goldfinches (Spinus tristis; Pham et al. 2014), and Common Moorhen (Gallinula chloropus) females increase frontal shield size in response to testosterone implants (Eens et al. 2000), demonstrating that androgens are a key mediator of ornament expression in both sexes.

While Pūkeko have significant testicular asymmetry, we found no relationship between this asymmetry and any aspect of ornamentation. Our results are consistent with 3 other studies that failed to find a relationship between testicular asymmetry and aspects of quality in birds (Birkhead et al. 1998, Kimball et al. 1997, Merilä and Sheldon 1999). Additionally, an experiment by Birkhead et al. (1998) showed that increased testicular asymmetry was associated with poor, rather than good, body condition in Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Indeed, the available evidence suggests that a positive relationship between testicular asymmetry and quality is not general across birds, and we have a poor understanding of the factors contributing to intra- and interspecific variation in testicular asymmetry (Calhim and Montgomerie 2015).

Bare-part ornaments have the potential to be highly informative signals because of their ability to respond rapidly to changes in individual physiology (e.g., Faivre et al. 2003, Ardia et al. 2010), which can be driven by changes in social or environmental conditions. Indeed, a recent experiment demonstrated that Pūkeko frontal shields can decrease in size within a 7-day period in response to increased aggressive challenges from group-mates (Dey et al. 2014a), which may provide a mechanism that tightly links signal expression to an individual's dominance rank. Many bare-part ornaments respond rapidly to changes in androgen levels (Allee et al. 1939, Eens et al. 2000, Ardia et al. 2010), which influence, and are influenced by, social interactions (Hirschenhauser and Oliveira 2006). As a result, androgen hormones may provide a general mechanism by which social interactions can indirectly influence signal phenotypes.

While Pūkeko are known to respond to experimental manipulations of frontal shield size, further study is required to determine whether they perceive and respond to natural variation in shield or breast plumage color. Interestingly, breast plumage coloration was found to reflect UV light (Figure 2), which is thought to be a ‘special' signaling channel for birds because it cannot be perceived by mammalian predators (Hausmann et al. 2003). Although rails do not possess the UV-sensitive cones found in most passerines (Ödeen and Håstad 2013), they can still perceive shorter wavelength light than humans because avian corneas and lenses are transparent to UV-A light (Cuthill 2006). As a result, Pūkeko may represent an interesting model for studies that compare UV (breast plumage) and non-UV (frontal shield) coloration.

In cooperatively breeding animals, dominance relationships can have a strong influence on the allocation of reproduction and individual fitness (Hauber and Lacey 2005, Nonacs and Hager 2011). Although dominance hierarchies could be maintained by individual recognition, many cooperatively breeding species also use morphological traits to signal and maintain dominance relationships (e.g., Tibbetts and Izzo 2010, Rat et al. 2015). In this study, we demonstrated that frontal shield size and color, as well as breast plumage color, were correlated with social dominance in the Pūkeko. However, frontal shield size was more informative across a range of analyses, demonstrating that different ornament types (e.g., bare-part vs. plumage traits) may provide differing amounts of information on individual quality (Møller and Pomiankowski 1993). These results therefore contribute to our understanding of multiple ornamentation in animals, and suggest that an increased focus on bare-part ornamentation in birds may advance the study of animal communication.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank Tawharanui staff and the Tawharanui Open Sanctuary Society for facilitating our research. We are very grateful to the Manu Huruhuru Wānanga (run by Te Ao Rosieur) under the Manuhiri Kaitiaki Charitable Trust and supported by whānau whānui of Ngati Manuhiri and the local community for helping us gather data on culled specimens. We also thank Adam Snowball, Andrew Green, Constance O'Connor, Diane Field, and Beth Nagai for assistance with data collection. Adam Reddon and Aneesh Bose provided helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. We dedicate this paper to a friend, Ian Jamieson, who recently passed away. Ian had a productive career as a behavioral and conservation biologist and performed many pioneering studies on Pūkeko. He will be missed by many in the science community.

Funding statement: This research was funded by an NSERC Canada Graduate Scholarship to C.J.D., an NSERC Discovery grant to J.S.Q., and Massey University funding to J.D.. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. None of the funders had any input into the content of the manuscript, nor required their approval of the manuscript before submission or publication.

Ethics statement: All applicable international, national, and institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. Animal trapping and handling protocols were approved through permits from the McMaster University Animal Research Ethics Board (#13-10-37), the New Zealand Department of Conservation (NZDOC) Wildlife Authority (39641-FAU), and the NZDOC High Impact Research and Collection Permit (35048-FAU).

Author contributions: This study was conceived by C.J.D., J.D., and J.S.Q. Data was collected by C.J.D., J.S.Q., A.K., J.D., and J.H. Data was analyzed by C.J.D. and J.D., and the paper was written by C.J.D., J.S.Q., and J.D.

LITERATURE CITED

1.

Allee, W. C., N. E. Collias, and C. Z. Lutherman (1939). Modification of the social order in flocks of hens by the injection of testosterone propionate. Physiological Zoology 12:412–440. Google Scholar

2.

Alvarez, F., C. Sánchez, and S. Angulo (2005). The frontal shield of the Moorhen: Sex differences and relationship with body condition. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 17:135–148. Google Scholar

3.

Ardia, D. R., D. R. Broughton, and M. J. Gleicher (2010). Short-term exposure to testosterone propionate leads to rapid bill color and dominance changes in Zebra Finches. Hormones and Behavior 58:526–532. Google Scholar

4.

Bartoń, K. (2015). MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R package version 1.15.6.  https://cran.r-project.org/package=MuMIn Google Scholar

5.

Bates, D., M. Mächler, B. M. Bolker, and S. C. Walker (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67:1–48.  10.18637/jss.v067.i01 Google Scholar

6.

Birkhead, T. R., F. Fletcher, and E. J. Pellatt (1998). Testes asymmetry, condition and sexual selection in birds: An experimental test. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 265:1185–1189. Google Scholar

7.

Bright, A., and J. R. Waas (2002). Effects of bill pigmentation and UV reflectance during territory establishment in Blackbirds. Animal Behaviour 64:207–213. Google Scholar

8.

Briskie, J. V., and R. Montgomerie (2007). Testis size, sperm size and sperm competition. InReproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Birds, Part A: Phylogeny, Morphology, Hormones, Fertilization ( B. G. M. Jamieson, Editor). CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, USA. pp. 513–551. Google Scholar

9.

Buchholz, R. (1995). Female choice, parasite load and male ornamentation in wild Turkeys. Animal Behaviour 50:929–943. Google Scholar

10.

Calhim, S., and T. R. Birkhead (2009). Intraspecific variation in testis asymmetry in birds: Evidence for naturally occurring compensation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 276:2279–2284. Google Scholar

11.

Calhim, S., and R. Montgomerie (2015). Testis asymmetry in birds: The influences of sexual and natural selection. Journal of Avian Biology 46:175–185. Google Scholar

12.

Chaine, A. S., and B. E. Lyon (2008). Adaptive plasticity in female mate choice dampens sexual selection on male ornaments in the Lark Bunting. Science 319:459–462. Google Scholar

13.

Chaine, A. S., K. A. Tjernell, D. Shizuka, and B. E. Lyon (2011). Sparrows use multiple status signals in winter social flocks. Animal Behaviour 81:447–453. Google Scholar

14.

Craig, J. L. (1977). The behaviour of the Pukeko, Porphyrio porphyrio melanotus. New Zealand Journal of Zoology 4:413–433. Google Scholar

15.

Craig, J. L. (1980). Pair and group breeding behaviour of a communal gallinule, the Pukeko, Porphyrio p. melanotus. Animal Behaviour 28:593–603. Google Scholar

16.

Craig, J. L., B. H. McArdle, and P. D. Wettin (1980). Sex determination of the Pukeko or Purple Swamphen. Notornis 27:287–291. Google Scholar

17.

Crowley, C. E., and R. D. Magrath (2004). Shields of offence: Signalling competitive ability in the Dusky Moorhen, Gallinula tenebrosa. Australian Journal of Zoology 52:463–474. Google Scholar

18.

Cuthill, I. C. (2006). Color perception. InBird Coloration, Volume 1: Mechanisms and Measurements ( G. E. Hilland K. J. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA. pp. 3–40. Google Scholar

19.

Dale, J. (2006). Intraspecific variation in coloration. InBird Coloration, Volume 2: Function and Evolution ( G. E. Hilland K. J. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA. pp. 36–86. Google Scholar

20.

David, H. A. (1987). Ranking from unbalanced paired-comparison data. Biometrika 74:432–436. Google Scholar

21.

Dey, C., and I. Jamieson (2013). Pukeko. InNew Zealand Birds Online ( C. M. Miskelly, Editor).  http://nzbirdsonline.org.nz/species/pukeko Google Scholar

22.

Dey, C. J., and J. S. Quinn (2014). Individual attributes and self-organizational processes affect dominance network structure in Pukeko. Behavioral Ecology 25:1402–1408. Google Scholar

23.

Dey, C. J., J. Dale, and J. S. Quinn (2014a). Manipulating the appearance of a badge of status causes changes in true badge expression. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 281:20132680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2680 Google Scholar

24.

Dey, C. J., I. G. Jamieson, and J. S. Quinn (2012). Reproductive skew and female trait elaboration in a cooperatively breeding rail. Ibis 154:452–460. Google Scholar

25.

Dey, C. J., C. M. O'Connor, S. Balshine,and J. S. Quinn (2014b). Cooperative males reduce incubation in response to cues of female–female competition. Ibis 156:446–451. Google Scholar

26.

Dey, C. J., M. Valcu, B. Kempenaers, and J. Dale (2015). Carotenoid-based bill coloration functions as a social, not sexual, signal in songbirds (Aves: Passeriformes). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 28:250–258. Google Scholar

27.

Eens, M., E. Van Duyse, L. Berghman, and R. Pinxten (2000). Shield characteristics are testosterone-dependent in both male and female Moorhens. Hormones and Behavior 37:126–134. Google Scholar

28.

Emlen, S. T., and P. H. Wrege (2004). Size dimorphism, intrasexual competition, and sexual selection in Wattled Jacana (Jacana jacana), a sex-role-reversed shorebird in Panama. The Auk 121:391–403. Google Scholar

29.

Faivre, B., A. Grégoire, M. Préault, F. Cézilly, and G. Sorci (2003). Immune activation rapidly mirrored in a secondary sexual trait. Science 300:103. Google Scholar

30.

Fenoglio, S., M. Cucco, and G. Malacarne (2002). Bill colour and body condition in the Moorhen Gallinula chloropus. Bird Study 49:89–92. Google Scholar

31.

Garamszegi, L. Z., M. Eens, S. Hurtrez-Boussès, and A. P. Møller (2005). Testosterone, testes size, and mating success in birds: A comparative study. Hormones and Behavior 47:389–409. Google Scholar

32.

Gelman, A. (2008). Scaling regression inputs by dividing by two standard deviations. Statistics in Medicine 27:2865–2873. Google Scholar

33.

Gullion G. W. (1951). The frontal shield of the American Coot. Wilson Bulletin 63:157–166. Google Scholar

34.

Hauber, M. E., and E. A. Lacey (2005). Bateman's principle in cooperatively breeding vertebrates: The effects of non-breeding alloparents on variability in female and male reproductive success. Integrative and Comparative Biology 45:903–914. Google Scholar

35.

Hausmann, F., K. E. Arnold, N. J. Marshall, and I. P. F. Owens (2003). Ultraviolet signals in birds are special. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 270:61–67. Google Scholar

36.

Hegyi, G., M. Laczi, G. Nagy, E. Szász, D. Kötél, and J. Török (2015). Stable correlation structure among multiple plumage colour traits: Can they work as a single signal?Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 114:92–108. Google Scholar

37.

Hill, G. E. (2006). Female mate choice for ornamental coloration. InBird Coloration, Volume 2: Function and Evolution ( G. E. Hilland K. J. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA. pp. 137–200. Google Scholar

38.

Hill, G. E., and K. J. McGraw (Editors)(2006). Bird Coloration, Volume 2: Function and Evolution. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA. Google Scholar

39.

Hirschenhauser, K., and R. F. Oliveira (2006). Social modulation of androgens in male vertebrates: Meta-analyses of the challenge hypothesis. Animal Behaviour 71:265–277. Google Scholar

40.

Jamieson, I. G. (1997). Testing reproductive skew models in a communally breeding bird, the Pukeko, Porphyrio porphyrio. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 264:335–340. Google Scholar

41.

Jamieson, I. G., and J. L. Craig (1987). Dominance and mating in a communal polygynandrous bird: Cooperation or indifference towards mating competitors?Ethology 75:317–327. Google Scholar

42.

Jamieson, I. G., J. S. Quinn, P. A. Rose, and B. N. White (1994). Shared paternity among non-relatives is a result of an egalitarian mating system in a communally breeding bird, the Pukeko. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 257:271–277. Google Scholar

43.

Jawor, J. M., N. Gray, S. M. Beall, and R. Breitwisch (2004). Multiple ornaments correlate with aspects of condition and behaviour in female Northern Cardinals, Cardinalis cardinalis. Animal Behaviour 67:875–882. Google Scholar

44.

Jawor, J. M., S. U. Linville, S. M. Beall, and R. Breitwisch (2003). Assortative mating by multiple ornaments in Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis). Behavioral Ecology 14:515–520. Google Scholar

45.

Johnson, K., D. Rosetta, and D. N. Burley (1993). Preferences of female American Goldfinches (Carduelis tristis) for natural and artificial male traits. Behavioral Ecology 4:138–143. Google Scholar

46.

Joslyn, W. D. (1973). Androgen-induce social dominance in infant female rhesus monkeys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 14:137–145. Google Scholar

47.

Kimball, R. T., J. D. Ligon, and M. Merola-Zwartjes (1997). Testicular asymmetry and secondary sexual characters in Red Junglefowl. The Auk 114:221–228. Google Scholar

48.

Kimball, R. T., and J. D. Ligon (1999). Evolution of avian plumage dichromatism from a proximate perspective. The American Naturalist 154:182–193. Google Scholar

49.

Kuznetsova, A., P. B. Brockhoff, and R. H. B. Christensen (2015). lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 2.0-30.  https://cran.r-project.org/package=lmerTest Google Scholar

50.

Laucht, S., and J. Dale (2012). Correlations of condition, testosterone, and age with multiple ornaments in male House Sparrows: Patterns and implications. The Condor 114:865–873. Google Scholar

51.

Ligon, J. D., R. Thornhill, M. Zuk, and K. Johnson (1990). Male-male competition, ornamentation and the role of testosterone in sexual selection in Red Jungle Fowl. Animal Behaviour 40:367–373. Google Scholar

52.

Mateos, C., and J. Carranza (1997). The role of bright plumage in male–male interactions in the Ring-necked Pheasant. Animal Behaviour 54:1205–1214. Google Scholar

53.

Mercadante, A., and G. E. Hill (2014). An experimental test of the role of structural blue and melanin-based chestnut coloration in aggressive contests in male Eastern Bluebirds. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 2:24.  10.3389/fevo.2014.00024 Google Scholar

54.

Merilä, J., and B. C. Sheldon (1999). Testis size variation in the Greenfinch Carduelis chloris: Relevance for some recent models of sexual selection. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 45:115–123. Google Scholar

55.

Møller, A. P. (1994). Directional selection on directional asymmetry: Testes size and secondary sexual characters in birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 258:147–151. Google Scholar

56.

Møller, A. P., and A. Pomiankowski (1993). Why have birds got multiple sexual ornaments?Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 32:167–176. Google Scholar

57.

Montgomerie, R. (2006). Analyzing colors. InBird Coloration, Volume 1: Mechanisms and Measurements ( G. E. Hilland K. J. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA. pp. 90–147. Google Scholar

58.

Moss, R., H. H. Kolb, M. Marquiss, A. Watson, B. Treca, D. Watt, and W. Glennie (1979). Aggressiveness and dominance in captive cock Red Grouse. Aggressive Behavior 5:59–84. Google Scholar

59.

Murphy, T. G., M. F. Rosenthal, R. Montgomerie, and K. A. Tarvin (2009). Female American Goldfinches use carotenoid-based bill coloration to signal status. Behavioral Ecology 20:1348–1355. Google Scholar

60.

Nakagawa, S., and I. C. Cuthill (2007). Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: A practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews 82:591–605. Google Scholar

61.

Nakagawa, S., and H. Schielzeth (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 4:133–142. Google Scholar

62.

Nonacs, P., and R. Hager (2011). The past, present and future of reproductive skew theory and experiments. Biological Reviews 86:271–298. Google Scholar

63.

Ödeen, A., and O. Håstad (2013). The phylogenetic distribution of ultraviolet sensitivity in birds. BMC Evolutionary Biology 13:36.  10.1186/1471-2148-13-36 Google Scholar

64.

Omland, K. E. (1996). Female Mallard mating preferences for multiple male ornaments: II. Experimental variation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 39:361–366. Google Scholar

65.

Parker, G. A. (1974). Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology 47:223–243. Google Scholar

66.

Parker, T. H., and J. D. Ligon (2003). Female mating preferences in Red Junglefowl: A meta-analysis. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 15:63–72. Google Scholar

67.

Petrie, M. (1988). Intraspecific variation in structures that display competitive ability: Large animals invest relatively more. Animal Behaviour 36:1174–1179. Google Scholar

68.

Pham, T. T., P. S. Queller, K. A. Tarvin, and T. G. Murphy (2014). Honesty of a dynamic female aggressive status signal: Baseline testosterone relates to bill color in female American Goldfinches. Journal of Avian Biology 45:22–28. Google Scholar

69.

Quesada, J., and J. C. Senar (2006). Comparing plumage colour measurements obtained directly from live birds and from collected feathers: The case of the Great Tit Parus major. Journal of Avian Biology 37:609–616. Google Scholar

70.

Quinn, J. S., J. Haselmayer, C. Dey, and I. G. Jamieson (2012). Tolerance of female co-breeders in joint-laying Pukeko: The role of egg recognition and peace incentives. Animal Behaviour 83:1035–1041. Google Scholar

71.

Ramos-Fernández, G., J. L. Mateos, O. Miramontes, G. Cocho, H. Larralde, and B. Ayala-Orozco (2004). Lévy walk patterns in the foraging movements of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 55:223–230. Google Scholar

72.

Rat, M., R. E. van Dijk, R. Covas, and C. Doutrelant (2015). Dominance hierarchies and associated signalling in a cooperative passerine. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 69:437–448. Google Scholar

73.

R Core Team(2015). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.  http://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar

74.

Rohwer, S. (1975). The social significance of avian winter plumage variability. Evolution 29:593–610. Google Scholar

75.

Rohwer, S. (1982). The evolution of reliable and unreliable badges of fighting ability. American Zoologist 22:531–546. Google Scholar

76.

Santos, E. S. A., D. Scheck, and S. Nakagawa (2011). Dominance and plumage traits: Meta-analysis and metaregression analysis. Animal Behaviour 82:3–19. Google Scholar

77.

Schenker, N., and J. F. Gentleman (2001). On judging the significance of differences by examining the overlap between confidence intervals. American Statistician 55:182–186. Google Scholar

78.

Senar, J. C. (2006). Color displays as intrasexual signals of aggression and dominance. InBird Coloration, Volume 2: Function and Evolution ( G. E. Hilland K. J. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA. pp. 87–136. Google Scholar

79.

Shawcross, J. E., and P. J. B. Slater (1984). Agonistic experience and individual recognition in male Quelea quelea. Behavioural Processes 9:49–60. Google Scholar

80.

Taves, M. D., J. K. Desjardins, S. Mishra, and S. Balshine (2009). Androgens and dominance: Sex-specific patterns in a highly social fish (Neolamprologus pulcher). General and Comparative Endocrinology 161:202–207. Google Scholar

81.

Tibbetts, E. A. (2014). The evolution of honest communication: Integrating social and physiological costs of ornamentation. Integrative and Comparative Biology 54:578–590. Google Scholar

82.

Tibbetts, E. A., and A. Izzo (2010). Social punishment of dishonest signalers caused by mismatch between signal and behavior. Current Biology 20:1637–1640. Google Scholar

83.

Tibbetts, E. A., and R. J. Safran (2009). Co-evolution of plumage characteristics and winter sociality in New and Old World sparrows. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 22:2376–2386. Google Scholar

84.

Torres, R., and A. Velando (2003). A dynamic trait affects continuous pair assessment in the Blue-footed Booby, Sula nebouxii. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 55:65–72. Google Scholar

85.

Valcu, M., and J. Dale (2014). colorZapper: Color extraction utilities. R package version 1.0.  https://github.com/valcu/colorZapper/ Google Scholar

86.

Vitousek, M. N., D. M. Zonana, and R. J. Safran (2014). An integrative view of the signaling phenotype: Dynamic links between signals, physiology, behavior and social context. Current Zoology 60:739–754. Google Scholar

87.

West-Eberhard, M. J. (1983). Sexual selection, social competition, and speciation. Quarterly Review of Biology 58:155–183. Google Scholar

88.

Wickham, H. (2009). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer-Verlag, New York, NY, USA. Google Scholar
Cody J. Dey, James S. Quinn, Ash King, Jessica Hiscox, and James Dale "A bare-part ornament is a stronger predictor of dominance than plumage ornamentation in the cooperatively breeding Australian Swamphen," The Auk 134(2), 317-329, (18 January 2017). https://doi.org/10.1642/AUK-16-119.1
Received: 14 June 2016; Accepted: 1 October 2016; Published: 18 January 2017
KEYWORDS
aggression
badge of status
color
feather
multiple ornamentation
pūkeko
status signal
Back to Top