Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2005 Spacing and activity patterns of leopards Panthera pardus in the Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal
Morten Odden, Per Wegge
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Space use and activity of radio-collared leopards Panthera pardus (two adult males and one adult female) were monitored during 3–25 months in a prey-rich part of the Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal. Annual home ranges of the two males were 47 and 48 km2 and had an overlap of only 7%, whereas the overlap between the female's home range (17 km2) and that of one of the males was 56%. The range sizes were larger than reported from other studies in southeast Asia, but much smaller than some ranges in Africa. When comparing different studies, the sexual difference in range size increased significantly with increasing average range size. Thus, the cost by males of traversing large home ranges is probably not a determinant factor in shaping leopard communities. The female's seasonal home ranges (5.2 and 6.6 km2) were smallest during the seasons when her cubs were less than six months of age. She moved her home range closer to agricultural fields during the season when the abundant and important prey axis deer Axis axis visits these areas most frequently. No such pattern was detected among the two males. Instead they frequented human settlements throughout the year, probably in order to hunt easily accessible domestic animals. Home ranges in similar seasons in consecutive years overlapped more (female = 64%, male = 75%) than ranges in different seasons in the same year (female = 38%, male = 64%). Intensive tracking sessions of 24 hours revealed that the diel activity levels of the two sexes were similar (female: 62.3%, males: 62.6%). However, their patterns of activity were different as the males moved mainly at night (day: 1,582 m, night: 5,244 m) and the female moved similar distances day and night (day: 2,381 m, night: 2,698 m). The female may have restricted her movement at the time when conspecific males were likely to be active in order to protect her cubs from infanticide. The males moved in a more linear manner than the female, and the linear distances between radio locations from consecutive days differed significantly between the sexes (male: 3,324 m, female: 881 m), but the actual distances moved during the 24-hour cycles were fairly similar (male: 6,826 m, female: 5,079 m).

Previous radio-telemetry studies of leopards Panthera pardus have revealed pronounced variations in spacing patterns between different study areas. The variation is particularly evident when comparing home range sizes, which range from 6 km2 (Seidensticker et al. 1990) to more than 2,000 km2 (Bothma et al. 1997). Moreover, the degree of overlap between neighbours has been described as both extensive (Stander et al. 1997) and small (Rabinowitz 1989, Seidensticker et al. 1990, Mizutani & Jewell 1998). The variability among the previous studies is probably related to differences in the spatial distribution of food resources, as the density and movement of prey are essential determinants of predator behaviour (Schaller 1972, Malcolm & Van Lawick 1975, Frame et al. 1979, Bailey 1993). The variation may also be due to small sample sizes, or due to differences in sex, reproductive and social status and metabolic needs (Gittleman & Harvey 1982, Bailey 1993).

In this study, we describe the spacing and activity patterns of leopards in a prey-rich area of lowland Nepal and relate intersexual differences to season, prey conditions and reproductive status. We also evaluate home range estimators and attempt to identify a general trend in spacing pattern by comparing our findings with those from other studies.

Material and methods

Our study was conducted within a 100-km2 area in the southwestern part of the Royal Bardia National Park (986 km2). The park is situated in southwest Nepal where the foothills of the Himalayas flatten out on the Indian lowlands (about 100 m a.s.l.). The climate is subtropical monsoonal with heavy rains from July to September/October. Sal forest, interspersed with patches of Imperata cylindrica-dominated grasslands, and riverine forest and tallgrass flood plains are the dominating plant associations (Dinerstein 1979a, Sharma 1999). Of the 32 different mammal species recorded in the Park (Dinerstein 1979b), axis deer Axis axis contributes nearly 90% of the herbivore biomass (Andersen & Næss 1993). Other commonly occurring prey species are hog deer Axis porcinus, barking deer Muntiacus muntjak, wild boar Sus scrofa, and barasingha Cervus duvauceli. Until the late 1970s, a large number of domestic animals grazed within the park, but since then grazing has virtually ceased. Ungulate prey density is high with > 200 animals/km2 within the study area (Andersen & Næss 1993, Støen & Wegge 1996). Tiger Panthera tigris is the dominant carnivore. Leopard coexists at lower density, and sloth bear Melursus ursinus, striped hyena Hyaena hyaena, and dhole Cuon alpinus occur in still fewer numbers (Heinen & Kattel 1992).

Three adult leopards, two males (M1 and M2) and one female (F1), were captured in box traps and immobilised with a combination of 8.7–10.4 mg/kg ketamine and 4.35–6.2 mg/kg xylazine in three captures, and a mixture of 3.6-5.9 mg/kg ketamine and 0.07-0.12 mg/kg medetomidine in two captures. The sedated animals were weighed, measured, fitted with radio collars in the 164-MHz frequency range and aged according to Bailey (1993). After handling, all animals were given an antidote (atipamezole) and monitored from a distance until they were mobile. Morphometric data and tracking data are listed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Morphometric and tracking data of three adult leopards (one female, F1, and two males, M1 and M2) studied in the Royal Bardia National Park during 1999–2001.

i0909-6396-11-2-145-t01.gif

The leopards were located by triangulation from the ground using portable receivers and three-element Yagi antennas. At each compass bearing we determined whether the animal was active or passive based on variation in the signal strength. The range of the radio signals rarely exceeded 700 m, probably due to the dense vegetation and the flat topography within the study area. Nevertheless, locating the animals was usually not difficult, since several roads encompassed the study area and the home ranges of the leopards were relatively small. The short distance between the animal and the observer ensured a high level of precision; nearly all error polygons were within < 0.5 ha.

We conducted 24-hour continuous tracking sessions on 19 occasions (F1 = 13, M1 = 4, M2 = 2), during which activity was recorded every 15 minutes and precise radio fixes were obtained every hour. In the analysis of these data we defined the period between 06:00 and 18:00 as 'day', and the period between 18:00 and 06:00 as 'night'.

All radio locations were plotted on 1:10,000 topographic maps and digitised. Digitising, home range estimation and movement analysis were conducted using ArcView GIS 3.2. We used minimum convex polygon (MCP; Mohr 1947) and fixed kernel (95%, least squares cross validation and individual smoothing factor; Worton 1989) as home range estimators. We used bootstrap simulations to examine the relationship between sample size and home range size when using MCP and fixed kernel. In bootstrap simulations, range size is estimated from an increasing number of randomly selected radio locations. We started with 10 locations and increased the sample size with 10 until all radio locations were included in the calculations. We did five repetitions in each interval.

Seasonal home ranges were calculated whenever > 15 radio locations had been retrieved during a time span of more than one month during one season. We defined the period from November to March as the cool dry season, from March to July as the hot dry season and from July to November as the wet season. Overlap was calculated according to Minta (1993). Seasonal occupancy centres were calculated by averaging the coordinates of all the locations obtained during each season. We analysed day-to-day movement by measuring the distance between locations obtained on consecutive days. In all home range estimates and day-to-day movement analyses we used no more than one location per day. We used the location taken nearest 12:00 whenever more than one location had been obtained on a single day. Diel movement distances were calculated by adding up the distances between consecutive locations that were obtained during the 24-hour intensive tracking sessions.

In all tables and analyses which include home range data from previous studies only adult individuals were considered. If several methods had been used, we chose kernel home range estimates if available; otherwise we chose MCP. In the comparisons of sexual differences in home range size between the study areas we used the following definitions: sexual difference in range size was determined as the average range size of males divided by the average range size of females. Average range size was determined as the average range size of males + the average range size of females divided by two.

Results

Spacing pattern

The home ranges of the three leopards were located along the border of the park and consisted predominantly of sal forests with small patches of grasslands. The range size of the female leopard (F1) calculated by the kernel estimator was 16.9 km2 when including the tracking data from the whole study period (hereafter termed 'total home range'), whereas the total ranges of the males, M1 and M2, were 46.8 km2 and 48.1 km2, respectively (Table 2). The home range sizes of F1 and M1 were 17.1 km2 and 50.4 km2, respectively, when using the MCP as estimator and thus fairly similar to the ones obtained using the kernel method. In contrast, the size of M2's total home range was only 20.1 km2 with MCP, which is more than two times smaller than the kernel estimate. As seen in Figure 1, the MCP method is sensitive to small sample size, and as a consequence, the estimated home range of M2 is more reliable when using the kernel method.

Table 2.

Estimated home range sizes (in km2) of radio-collared leopards in Southeast Asia.

i0909-6396-11-2-145-t02.gif

Figure 1.

Relationship between sample size and home range size using MCP (100%) and fixed kernel methods (95%), respectively, as estimators for the female (F1) and the two male (M1 and M2) leopards in the Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal.

i0909-6396-11-2-145-f01.gif

Even though the home ranges of the leopards in our study were relatively small compared to those of leopards living in more arid environments in Africa (Table 3), they were the largest ever recorded in southeast Asia (see Table 2). The average size of the home ranges of the males in our study was 2.8 times larger than the range of the female, whereas the corresponding values from previous studies average 3.0 (SD = 0.9, N = 7; see Table 3). When comparing different study areas, the sexual difference in range size was significantly correlated with the average range size (Spearman Rank Correlation Test, two tailed: r = 0.762, N = 8, P < 0.05; Fig. 2). The positive correlation implies that the sexual difference in range size becomes larger when the average range sizes increase.

Table 3.

Average home range sizes (in km2) of adult leopards in this and previous studies.

i0909-6396-11-2-145-t03.gif

Figure 2.

Relationship between sexual differences in home range size for male/females and the average home range size (male+female/2) in our study (1) and the following previous leopard studies: Rabinowitz 1989 (2), Grassman 1999 (3), Jenny 1996 (4), Stander et al. 1997 (5), Mizutani & Jewell 1998 (6), Bailey 1993 (7) and Bothma et al. 1997a (8).

i0909-6396-11-2-145-f02.gif

The overlap between M1 and M2 was 7% when using 95% fixed kernel as home range estimator. The overlap between F1 and M1 was 56%, but between F1 and M2 there was no overlap. Hence, the spatial distribution of the ranges was in accordance with what should be expected for a solitary felid as there was a pronounced sexual difference in home range size, a high degree of intersexual overlap, and a small degree of intra-sexual overlap (Fig. 3). Pugmark tracking and camera trapping indicated that there were no resident males located between M1 and M2 (Eliassen 2003), which would have increased the degree of overlap between the males.

Figure 3.

Total home range boundaries using 95% kernel of one adult female (F1 = ___) and two adult male (M1 = ..... and M2 = - - -) leopards in the Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal, and seasonal occupancy centres of the female.

i0909-6396-11-2-145-f03.gif

The annual and seasonal home ranges of F1 and M1 varied in size (Table 4). The variation was probably not due to sampling as the bootstrap simulations showed that the kernel estimator produced fairly similar-sized ranges in the whole array of sample sizes. F1 gave birth to two cubs in late June of 1999 and two cubs in August of 2000. Hence, the smallest seasonal home ranges of F1 during the wet season of 1999, the wet season of 2000 and the cool season of 2000, coincide with the time when her cubs were < 6 months of age.

Table 4.

Annual and seasonal home range sizes (expressed as fixed kernel (95%, in km2) of two adult leopards (female F1 and male M1) in the Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal.

i0909-6396-11-2-145-t04.gif

Seasonal range overlap was estimated for F1 and M1 separately (Table 5A). Range overlap between different seasons was smaller than the overlap between similar seasons in consecutive years. Also, the distances between the occupancy centres of similar seasons were shorter than the distances between different seasons in both M1 and F1 (Table 5B). For M2 we had too few data for it to be included.

Table 5.

Overlap (in %) between seasonal home ranges (using kernel 95%; A) and distance (in m) between seasonal occupancy centres (B) of a female (F1) and a male (M1) leopard in the Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal.

i0909-6396-11-2-145-t05.gif

F1 showed significant seasonal differences in the distances between radio fixes and the park border (Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA on ranks: H = 64.1, df = 2, P < 0.0001). A pairwise comparison revealed that F1 was significantly closer to the park border during the cool seasons than during the other seasons and most distant during the hot seasons (Dunn's method, cool seasons vs hot seasons: Q = 7.90, P < 0.05, cool seasons vs wet seasons: Q = 4.13, P < 0.05, hot seasons vs wet seasons: Q = 2.68, P < 0.05; Fig. 4). M1 showed no such seasonal difference in the distance to the park border (H = 2.59, df = 2, P = 0.2735). Contrary to F1, he seemed to be located close to the park border throughout the whole year (see Fig. 3). M2 was not included in the analysis due to lack of data.

Figure 4.

Distribution of the female F1's and the male M1's radio locations during different seasons in relation to the distance to the border of the Royal Bardia National Park.

i0909-6396-11-2-145-f04.gif

Movement and activity

The distance between locations obtained on consecutive days averaged 881 m for F1 (N = 164, SD = 893 m). The distance between locations of M1 (Mean = 3,324 m, N = 26, SD = 2,912 m) was significantly longer (Mann-Whitney: T = 3,579.0, P < 0.0001). M2 was not included due to lack of data.

F1 was active in 76% of the radio locations obtained during daytime (SD = 35%, N = 335), while M1 was active in 66% (SD = 41%, N = 99) and M2 was active in 73% (SD = 35%, N = 17). The diurnal level of activity differed significantly between the three leopards (H = 8.20, df = 2, P = 0.0166). F1 was significantly more active during daytime than M1 (Q = 2.82, P < 0.05), and the difference in activity level was least pronounced between the two males (Q = 0.466, not significant).

The 24-hour intensive tracking sessions (F1 = 13, M1= 4, M2 = 2) revealed that the two sexes had similar mean diel activity levels (t-test: t = 0.0798, P = 0.937); F1 was active in 62.3% of the measurements (SD = 8.28), and the males were active during 62.6% (SD = 8.51). The males' mean diel movement distance (6,826 m, SD= 3,510 m) was longer than the distance moved by F1 (5,079 m, SD = 2,133 m), but did not differ significantly (t = 1.34, P = 0.198). However, the two sexes moved at different times of the day. The female moved similar distances during the day and night (day: 2,381 m, SD = 1,923 m, night: 2,698 m, SD = 1,474 m, N = 13; t = 0.472, P = 0.641), whereas the males moved mainly during the night (day: 1,582 m, SD = 985 m, night: 5,244 m, SD = 3,441 m; T = 56.0, P = 0.004). Furthermore, F1 was never located outside of the park during the intensive tracking sessions, whereas M1 was outside the park border during three sessions and M2 during one session.

Discussion

Several authors have described an inverse relationship between food availability and territory size in territorial animals (e.g. Ebersole 1980, Hixon 1980, Schoener 1981, Saitoh 1991). This relationship seems evident when reviewing previous leopard studies conducted in Africa, where the drier habitats with a sparser prey distribution produce extremely large home ranges, and the more humid and prey-rich habitats produce smaller ranges. The difference in home range sizes in Asian studies is far less pronounced than in African studies, which is probably due to smaller differences in prey abundance between the study areas.

The sexual differences in home range size observed in our and previous leopard studies are considerably larger than expected from sexual differences in energy requirements of solitary carnivores (Sandell 1989). Hence, the basic social organisation of solitary felids, in which the limiting resource for females is access to food and the limiting resource for males is access to females, seems to be a common feature among leopards throughout their distribution range. Surprisingly, the sexual difference in home range size was significantly more pronounced in study areas where home ranges were large. Hence, males seem to expand their ranges proportionally more than females in areas where resources are more sparsely distributed. This implies that the cost by males of traversing large home ranges is not a determinant factor in shaping leopard communities.

Reproductive status seemed to affect the area use of F1, as her three smallest seasonal home ranges coincided with the time when her cubs were less than six months of age. This is in accordance with previous studies of leopard (Bailey 1993) and mountain lion Felis concolor (Hemker et al. 1984), which showed that rearing small cubs restricts movement of the mother.

Previous studies have demonstrated relationships between spatiotemporal changes in prey distribution and the movement patterns of leopards (Bailey 1993) and bobcats Felis rufus (Litvaitis et al. 1987, Koehler & Hornocker 1989). Accordingly, seasonal changes in the distribution of Axis deer, the main prey of leopards in our study area (Eliassen 2003), may explain why different parts of the total home range of our female leopard were more intensively used during different seasons. The recurrent movements of F1 close to the park border during the cool seasons (see Fig. 3) coincide with the time when axis deer visit the agricultural fields for foraging most frequently (Moe & Wegge 1994).

In contrast to F1, a large proportion of M1's radio locations were situated close to the park border in all seasons, not only during the cool season (see Fig. 3). The difference between the two leopards probably reflects the fact that male leopards are more prone to be stock-raiders than females (Hamilton 1981, Esterhuizen & Norton 1985). The 24-hour tracking sessions support this conclusion, since F1 was never located within human settlements in any of the 13 sessions, whereas the males were found in villages during four out of six sessions. According to Sukumar (1991), males of polygynous species more frequently cause conflicts with humans than females due to inherent higher risk-taking behaviour. The 'high risk-high gain' strategy is favoured among males of such species due to their greater variance in reproductive success (Trivers 1985).

An important and obvious factor affecting our results concerning space use was the choice of home range estimator. The MCP method, which has frequently been used as home range estimator in other leopard studies, is criticised for its sensitivity to small sample sizes, and because it usually incorporates large areas that are never used (Bekoff & Mech 1984, Powell 1987, White & Garrott 1990). Our bootstrap simulations and Mitzutani & Jewell's (1998) study confirmed that relatively large samples sizes were required to produce reliable home range estimates. Likewise, Bothma et al. (1997) concluded that the home range sizes of all nine leopards tracked in their study were underestimated when using MCP due to limited sample sizes. A common argument for choosing MCP is that it allows comparison with previous studies. In our opinion this argument is equivalent to saying that it is better to compare two unreliable estimates than comparing one good estimate with a bad one. We disagree with this argument and believe that other home range estimators, such as kernel methods, are more appropriate.

The sexual difference in day-to-day movement was 3.8:1, and thus, somewhat similar to the sexual difference in range size (2.8:1). Probably, the males moved longer distances every day in order to regularly frequent all parts of their larger territories. However, the intensive tracking sessions revealed that the actual distance moved during 24-hour periods differed to a lesser degree between the sexes. This implies that the primary cause of the pronounced sexual difference in day-to-day movement was a more linear movement pattern among the males.

The males were less active than the female during daytime. Moreover, the female moved similar distances during day and night, whereas males moved mainly at night. A more nocturnal behaviour in males was also found in studies of lynx Lynx lynx (Schmidt 1999) and bobcat (Wassmer et al. 1988). Seidensticker (1977) reported that a radio-marked female leopard rearing small cubs in the Chitwan National Park spent more time in the immediate vicinity of her cubs during night than during day. F1's cubs were 4-8 months old during the intensive tracking sessions, and her movement pattern seemed somewhat similar to the movements of the female in Chitwan. Infanticide has been recorded as a factor of mortality in previous studies of Carnivora (Swenson 2003), including solitary cats (leopards: Bailey 1993, Iliany 1990; mountain lion: Logan & Sweanor 2001) and inferred in tiger (Smith & McDougal 1991). In order to avoid infanticide F1 may have restricted her movement at night in order to be closer to her cubs at the time when conspecific males are more active.

Acknowledgments

Our study was funded by the Royal Norwegian Agency for Development Cooperation (NORAD) as part of a collaborative research programme between the Department of National Parks and Wildlife Conservation/Nepal and the Agricultural University of Norway (now the Norwegian University of Life Sciences). The Royal Bardia National Park (RBNP) and the King Mahendra Trust for Nature Conservation (KMTNC) provided technical support. In particular, we want to thank Narayan Choudary and Tanku Choudary (RBNP), Shanta Raj Jnawali, Man Bahadur Lama, Man Singh Lama and Suk Bahadur (KMTNC), Hilde Mosand, Trude Eliassen and Jon. M. Arnemo for their assistance in capturing and tracking animals in the field.

References

1.

H. J. Andersen and K. M. Næss . 1993. Assessing census techniques for wild ungulates in Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal M.Sc. thesis, Agricultural University of Norway. 101. pp.  Google Scholar

2.

T. N. Bailey 1993. The African leopard. Columbia University Press. New York. 429. pp.  Google Scholar

3.

M. Bekoff and L. D. Mech . 1984. Computer simulation: Simulation analyses of space use: home range estimates, variability, and sample size.-. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers 16:32–37. Google Scholar

4.

B. C. R. Bertram 1982. Leopard ecology as studied by radio tracking.-. Symposia of the Zoological Society of London 49:341–352. Google Scholar

5.

Jdu P. Bothma, M. H. Knight, E. A. N. leRiche, and H. J. Van Hensbergen . 1997. Range size of southern Kalahari leopards. South African Journal of Wildlife Research 27:394–99. Google Scholar

6.

E. Dinerstein 1979a. An ecological survey of the Royal Karnali-Bardia Wildlife Reserve, Nepal. Part I: Vegetation, modifying factors and successional relationships.-. Biological Conservation 15:127–149. Google Scholar

7.

E. Dinerstein 1979b. An ecological survey of the Royal Karnali-Bardia Wildlife Reserve, Nepal. Part II: Habitat/Animal interactions.-. Biological Conservation 16:265–300. Google Scholar

8.

S. P. Ebersole 1980. Food density and territory size: An alternative model and test on the reef fish Eupomacentrus leucostius.-. American Naturalist 115:492–509. Google Scholar

9.

T. Eliassen 2003. Niche separation and food competition between tigers (Panthera tigris) and leopards (Panthera pardus) in Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal. M.Sc. thesis, Agricultural University of Norway. 52. pp.  Google Scholar

10.

W. C. N. Esterhuizen and P. M. Norton . 1985. The leopard as a problem animal in the Cape Province, as determined by the permit system.-. Bontebok 4:9–16. Google Scholar

11.

L. H. Frame, J. R. Malcolm, G. W. Frame, and H. Van Lawick . 1979. Social organisation of African wild dogs (Lyacon pictus) on the Serengeti plains, Tanzania 1967–1978.-. Zeitscrift für Tierpsychologie 50:225–249. Google Scholar

12.

J. L. Gittleman and P. H. Harvey . 1982. Carnivore home range size, metabolic needs and ecology.-. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 10:57–63. Google Scholar

13.

L. I. Grassman 1999. Ecology and behaviour of the indochinese leopard in Kaeng Krachan National Park, Thailand. Natural History Bulletin of the Siam Society 47:177–93. Google Scholar

14.

P. H. Hamilton 1981. The leopard Panthera pardus and cheetah Acinonyx jubatus in Kenya: Ecology, status, conservation and management Unpublished report for the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, The African Wildlife Leadership Foundation and the Government of Kenya. 36. pp.  Google Scholar

15.

J. T. Heinen and B. Kattel . 1992. Parks, people and conservation: A review of management issues in Nepal's protected areas.-. Population and Environment 14:49–84. Google Scholar

16.

T. P. Hemker, F. G. Lindzey, and B. B. Ackerman . 1984. Population characteristics and movement patterns of cougars in Southern Utah. Journal of Wildlife Management 48:41275–1284. Google Scholar

17.

M. A. Hixon 1980. Food production and competitor density as the determinants of feeding territory size.-. American Naturalist 115:510–530. Google Scholar

18.

G. Iliany 1990. Leopard Panthera pardus in Israel.-. Cat news 12:4–5. Google Scholar

19.

D. Jenny 1996. Spatial organisation of leopards Panthera pardus in Taï National Park, Ivory Coast: is rainforest habitat a ‘tropical haven’? Journal of Zoology (London) 240:427–440. Google Scholar

20.

K. U. Karanth and M. E. Sunquist . 2000. Behavioural correlates of predation by tiger (Panthera tigris), leopard (Panthera pardus) and dhole (Cuon alpinus) in Nagarahole, India. Journal of Zoology (London) 250:255–265. Google Scholar

21.

G. M. Koehler and M. G. Hornocker . 1989. Influences of seasons on bobcats in Idaho. Journal of Wildlife Management 53:1197–202. Google Scholar

22.

P. G. Le Roux and J. D. Skinner . 1989. A note on the ecology the leopard (Panthera pardus Linneaus) in the Londolozi Game Reserve, South Africa.-. African Journal of Ecology 27:167–171. Google Scholar

23.

J. A. Litvaitis, J. T. Major, and J. A. Sherburne . 1987. Influence of seasons and human-induced mortality on spatial organisation of bobcats (Felis rufus) in Maine. Journal of Mammology 68:1100–106. Google Scholar

24.

K. A. Logan and L. L. Sweanor . 2001. Desert Puma: Evolutionary ecology and Conservation of an enduring carnivore. Island Press. Washington USA. 464. pp.  Google Scholar

25.

J. R. Malcolm and H. Van Lawick . 1975. Notes on wild dogs (Lyacon pictus) hunting zebras. Mammalia 39:2231–240. Google Scholar

26.

S. C. Minta 1993. Sexual differences in spatiotemporal interaction among badgers.-. Oecologia 96:402–409. Google Scholar

27.

F. Mizutani and P. A. Jewell . 1998. Home range and movements of leopards (Panthera pardus) on a livestock ranch in Kenya. Journal of Zoology (London) 244:269–286. Google Scholar

28.

S. Moe and P. Wegge . 1994. Spacing behaviour and habitat use of axis deer Axis axis in lowland Nepal.-. Canadian Journal of Zoology 72:1735–1744. Google Scholar

29.

C. O. Mohr 1947. Table of equivalent populations of North American small mammals.-. American Midland Naturalist 37:223–249. Google Scholar

30.

P. M. Norton and S. R. Henley . 1987. Home range and movement of male leopards in the Cederberg Wilderness Area, Cape Province.-. South African Journal of Wildlife Research 17:41–48. Google Scholar

31.

P. M. Norton and A. B. Lawson . 1985. Radio tracking of leopards and caracals in the Stellenbosch area, Cape Province. South African Journal of Wildlife Research 15:117–24. Google Scholar

32.

R. A. Powell 1987. Black bear home range overlap in North Carolina and the concept of home range applied to back bears.-. International Conference on Bear Research and Management 7:235–242. Google Scholar

33.

A. Rabinowitz 1989. The density and behaviour of large cats in a dry tropical forest mosaic in Huai Kha Khaeng Wildlife Sanctuary, Thailand. Natural History Bulletin of the Siam Society 37:2235–251. Google Scholar

34.

T. Saitoh 1991. The effects and limits of territoriality on population regulation in grey red-backed voles, Clethrionomys rufocanus bedfordiae.-. Research on Population Ecology 33:367–386. Google Scholar

35.

M. Sandell 1989. The mating tactics and spacing patterns of solitary carnivores. In: J. L. Gittleman , editor. (Ed.). Carnivore behaviour, ecology and evolution. vol 1:Chapman and Hall. London. pp. 164–182. Google Scholar

36.

G. B. Schaller 1972. The Serengeti lion. University of Chicago Press. Chicago. 480. pp.  Google Scholar

37.

T. W. Schoener 1981. An empirically based estimate of home range.-. Theoretical Population Biology 20:281–325. Google Scholar

38.

K. Schmidt 1999. Variation in daily activity of free-living Eurasian lynx (Lynx lynx) in Bialowieza Primeval Forest, Poland. Journal of Zoology (London) 249:417–425. Google Scholar

39.

J. Seidensticker 1977. Notes on the early maternal behavior of the leopard.-. Mammalia 41:111–113. Google Scholar

40.

J. Seidensticker, M. E. Sunquist, and C. McDougal . 1990. Leopards living at the edge of the Royal Chitwan National Park, Nepal. In: J. C. Daniel and J. S. Serrao , editors. (Eds.). Conservation in developing countries: Problems and prospects. Proceedings of the Centenary Seminar of the Bombay Natural History Society and Oxford University Press. Bombay. pp. 415–423. Google Scholar

41.

B. K. Sharma 1999. Wildlife habitat mapping by using geographical information systems (GIS) in Royal Bardia National Park in lowland Nepal. M.Sc. thesis, Agricultural University of Norway. 68. pp.  Google Scholar

42.

J. L. D. Smith and C. McDougal . 1991. The contribution of variance in lifetime reproduction to effective population size in tigers.-. Conservation Biology 5:484–490. Google Scholar

43.

P. E. Stander, P. J. Haden, Kaqece, and Ghau . 1997. The ecology of asociality in Namibian leopards. Journal of Zoology (London) 242:343–364. Google Scholar

44.

O-G. Støen and P. Wegge . 1996. Prey selection and prey removal by tiger (Panthera tigris) in lowland Nepal.-. Mammalia 60:363–373. Google Scholar

45.

R. Sukumar 1991. The management of large mammals in relation to male strategies and conflict with people.-. Biological Conservation 55:93–102. Google Scholar

46.

J. E. Swenson 2003. Implications of sexually selected infanticide for hunting of large carnivores. In: M. Festa-Bianchet and M. Apollinio , editors. (Eds.). Animal behavior and wildlife management. Island Press. Washington. pp. 171–189. Google Scholar

47.

R. L. Trivers 1985. Social evolution. Benjamin/Cummings. Menlo Park. California, 462. pp.  Google Scholar

48.

D. A. Wassmer, D. D. Guenther, and J. N. Layne . 1988. Ecology of the bobcat in South-Central Florida. Bulletin of the Florida State Museum. Biological Sciences 33:1–228. Google Scholar

49.

G. C. White and R. A. Garrott . 1990. Analysis of wildlife radio-tracking data. Academic Press. New York. 383. pp.  Google Scholar

50.

B. J. Worton 1989. Kernel methods for estimating the utilisation distribution in home range studies.-. Ecology 70:164–168. Google Scholar

Notes

[1] Communicated by Associate Editor: Asir J.T. Johnsingh

Morten Odden and Per Wegge "Spacing and activity patterns of leopards Panthera pardus in the Royal Bardia National Park, Nepal," Wildlife Biology 11(2), 145-152, (1 June 2005). https://doi.org/10.2981/0909-6396(2005)11[145:SAAPOL]2.0.CO;2
Received: 15 June 2003; Accepted: 1 April 2004; Published: 1 June 2005
KEYWORDS
activity
home range
leopard
Panthera pardus
spacing
Subtropical forest
Back to Top