Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2009 Does the Use of Playback Affect the Estimates of Numbers of Grey Partridge Perdix perdix?
Zbigniew Kasprzykowski, Artur Goławski
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Methods of estimating bird numbers differ with regard to the results obtained and time required. A usual method of counting grey partridge Perdix perdix consists in counting calling males in the period with their highest vocal activity. This method was tested in our study with the additional use of playback. The number of males calling after playback was significantly higher than before playback. We found a positive relationship between the increase in the number of males calling after playback and their maximum numbers at the study site. However, after playback, the mean number of recorded males was significantly smaller in sites with the presence of red fox Vulpes vulpes burrows than in locations where this predator was absent. Without playback, there was no difference in the number of males between these two types of sites. The results of this study show that the use of playback improves the ability to detect grey partridges (as was similarly found with many other species). The use of playback in estimating the numbers of grey partridge may also allow further analyses, on e.g. site selectivity.

Knowledge of the abundance of a population of birds is essential for studies of avian population biology, community ecology and conservation. One of the critical issues is, however, that species abundance (or density) estimation methods must be validated and reliable. Many techniques exist for acquiring estimates or relative indices of bird population sizes (e.g. Granholm 1983, Verner 1985, Zuberogoitia & Campos 1998, Tryjanowski et al. 2003). Several methods of determining population density may exist for one species, and the grey partridge Perdix perdix is a good example. The most popular methods include: a thorough search of the site (Bro et al. 2004, Salek et al. 2004), line transect sampling of calling birds (Rotella & Ratti 1986), strip-census estimates (Pepin & Birkan 1981) and counts with the help of a trained dog (Panek 1992). To estimate the numbers of this species, counting calling males in the period of their highest vocal activity is used very frequently (Panek 1998, Klansek 2002, Wübbenhorst & Leuschner 2006), but, to our knowledge, only one study has used playback (Schoppers 1996). This latter method is, however, used frequently for many other species, in order to study whether song playback influences settlement patterns (Hahn & Silverman 2007), territorial behaviour (Petrusková et al. 2007) or the numbers of birds captured during migration (Kearns et al. 1998). In addition, playback is employed to increase the detection of many secretive bird species (Conway et al. 1993, Zuberogoitia & Campos 1998, Brambilla & Rubolini 2004), such as Galliformes (Evans at al. 2007). Increasing detection, which is likely to result in increasing sample size, may further allow detection of minor differences, e.g. a ‘change’ of habitat selection when analysing data collected with and without the use of playback (Jenkins & Ormerod 2002, Surmacki 2003).

Because of the catastrophic decline of grey partridge populations in many European countries (Aebischer & Kavangh 1997), precise data on their population biology and ecology are also useful in managing populations of this species (Aebischer & Ewald 2004). Vocal activity could be used to estimate winter survival, when the number of families observed in autumn is compared with the number of birds calling the following spring (Klansek 2002). Furthermore, it could also be used to analyse the parameters of reproduction in a given year, when the spring numbers of calling males are compared with the number of family flocks observed in autumn (i.e. brood production rate; Potts 1986, Panek 2005).

In our study, we aimed to determine: 1) whether male partridges react to playback of a male territorial call, and 2) whether playback methods increase detection by comparing spontaneous vocal activity and playback response. Additionally, we analysed whether the estimated numbers of grey partridge differed according to the presence of a predator, comparing results with and without voice stimulation. Red fox Vulpes vulpes is a predator responsible for great losses in grey partridge broods (Tapper et al. 1996, Parish & Sotherton 2007). During recent years, the number of foxes in Poland has increased dramatically and this species' utilised burrows are currently often found in the farmland landscape (Panek & Bresiński 2002, Gołdyn et al. 2003).

Material and methods

Our study was conducted in extensively used farmland in central-eastern Poland. The study area was located northeast of Siedlce (52°12′N, 22°31′E). The use of pesticides and mineral fertilisers is relatively low, and narrow fields are separated with distinct, wide balks in this region (Golawski & Meissner 2008). We selected 57 listening points, from which we reported spontaneous vocal activity and responses to playbacks. Each listening point was defined as a circle of a ca 500 m radius around a study point. The defined radius of audibility guaranteed the detection of all active males, because a grey partridge call can be heard from a distance of 800 m (Wiegand 1980). We used a shorter distance than that suggested in the literature because of various kinds of extraneous noise (e.g. vehicle sounds, barking dogs) related to the small size of farms in eastern Poland, which could hamper the detection of more distant males. The calls of this species were audible from a maximum distance of ca 500 m in other studies of grey partridge populations in open farmland landscapes (Goławski & Kasprzykowski, unpubl. data). Because of the relatively low numbers of grey partridge at most of the listening points, it was possible to count all calling males within the audible distance. Large fields with winter (mainly rye) and spring cereals predominated around listening points within these areas. Fallows, meadows and pastures did not exceed 10% of the total area. Detection of calling birds was noted by two independent observers simultaneously and compared.

We selected observation points located at least 500 m from the closest farm buildings and woodlands, since the density of the grey partridge decreases as forest coverage increases (Panek 1994). Listening points were located at road sides or maximum 200 m from roads, and traffic intensity at the selected locations was low. We travelled by car between consecutive listening points, located at least 1.6km apart (similarly to Rotella & Ratti 1986). During one evening, depending on the time needed to travel between subsequent points, we listened at a maximum of five locations. A few days before the stimulation, we penetrated the area of audio monitoring to search for fox. We surveyed all open farmland habitats, paying particular attention to fallows and balks, where fox burrows are most often found. This procedure was conducted at all 57 listening points. The criterion of fox presence was finding utilised burrows with fresh tracks nearby and an intense scent from a burrow. In a given site, fox burrows (1–4 entries to the burrow up to 30 m apart) were always located in only one place. Penetration by foxes looking for food is most intensive in the vicinity of utilised burrows (Blanco 1986).

Fieldwork was conducted during the early spring of 2006 and 2007. In 2006, we performed counts between 3 and 27 April at 26 listening points, and in 2007, between 27 March and 23 April at 31 listening points. These dates were related to the retreat of snow cover, which in 2006 was later than in 2007. Because of the distinct influence of weather on the vocal activity of grey partridges (Panek 1998), we only conducted fieldwork in good weather, i.e. rain less and windless weather, and when temperatures were above 0°C.

We estimated the number of calling grey partridge males based on 5-minute stays at each listening point. During the first two minutes (pre-playback period), we listened for calls to estimate the numbers of males before stimulation. Then we played grey partridge calls from a tape for one minute, and conducted a second listening session during the last two minutes to estimate numbers after playback. During the control period, we noted the total number of calling males. Some individuals had moved between the pre-playback and after-playback period, but in all cases, when this was detected, it was accounted for when calculating the final count in order to avoid double counts. It was of course possible that some movement occurred and remained undetected. However, because the total number of males at a given point was rather small, we considered that the potential error in the total number of grey partridge males would be negligible. We played the call of a territorial grey partridge male from the recordings of Roché (1990). Two cycles of grey partridge male territorial calls were played during playback. Grey partridge males called most intensively shortly after sunset and at sunrise. The morning counts might have been biased by the intense song of skylark Alauda arvensis, which began during the period of partridge vocal activity (Panek 1998). Because partridge vocal activity is very similar in the morning and evening (Rotella & Ratti 1986, Panek 1998), we limited the studies to evening counts. Vocal activity of the partridge began ca 15 minutes after sunset (Rotella & Ratti 1986), and at higher partridge densities, neighbouring males started calling almost simultaneously. In the few cases where there was no spontaneous vocal activity of grey partridges during the pre-playback period (seven cases), we started playing the tape 25 minutes after sunset. We waited for 25 minutes to ensure that the birds would not begin vocalising spontaneously.

The Wilcoxon test (Z) was used to compare the numbers of grey partridge males before and after playback. The Mann-Whitney test (U) was used to compare numbers of partridges at listening points with and without the presence of red fox. To test the relationship between the number of partridges, the advancement of the season, and the sequence of listening points in an evening, we used Spearman Rank Correlation (Sokal & Rohlf 2001). Because the numbers of grey partridge did not differ between 2006 and 2007, data from both seasons were combined in the analysis (numbers before stimulation: N = 57, U = 0.65, P = 0.503; numbers after stimulation: N = 57, U = 0.79, P = 0.428). We used Statistica 6.0 (StatSoft 2003) software for the statistical tests.

Figure 1.

Comparison of the mean (fi01_123.gif) number of calling males of the grey partridge before and after playback. Boxes indicate SD, and bars show minimum and maximum values.

f01_123.eps

Results

Playback experiments

The mean number of calling grey partridge males in the pre-playback period was 1.5 + 1.4 (SD), while after the playback it was 2.0+1.7 (N = 57, Z = 3.49, P < 0.001; Fig. 1). The maximum number of grey partridges heard during five minutes at a site was 2.2 +1.7 males (N = 57). The number of males after playback decreased at five listening points, remained the same at 25, and increased at 27 points. Maximum numbers of calling males were not related to the time during the season (N = 57, rs = -0.13, P = 0.333) nor to the sequence of listening points checked during one evening (N = 57, rs = -0.10, P = 0.474). There was a positive and significant relationship between the number of males and the difference of calling males before and after playback (N = 57, rs = 0.43, P < 0.001).

Influence of fox burrows on grey partridge vocal activity before and after playback

When no fox burrows were found, 1.7 (N = 39) grey partridge males were recorded during the pre-playback period on average, whereas when fox was present, the average was 1.1 (N = 18). However, these differences were not statistically significant (N = 57, U = 1.30, P = 0.195; Fig. 2). After playback, the mean number of calling males was 2.4 with no foxes, but only 1.2 in sites with foxes; the difference being statistically significant (N = 57, U = 2.34, P = 0.019; see Fig. 2).

Figure 2.

Comparison of the mean (fi02_123.gif) number of calling males of the grey partridge before (white bars) and after (dashed bars) play-back at sites where no utilised fox burrows were found (fox absent) and with utilised burrows (fox present). Boxes indicate SD, and bars show minimum and maximum values.

f02_123.eps

Discussion

Playback stimulation increased the detection of male partridges by 33%. The use of this method to determine the number of grey partridges was already used by Schoppers (1996), who noted a 1.6 times increase, a close result compared to our result (1.3-fold increase). At least in western Poland, vocal activity of the grey partridge is relatively stable between mid-March and mid-April, and therefore, there is no reason to repeat counts in order to estimate acuracy (Panek 1998). We also found this as we did not detect any decrease in the number of calling male grey partridges with season or time. Increasing detection (and numbers) by using playback has been found in many other bird species. Increasing the detection of the water rail Rallus aquaticus enabled researchers to determine its actual habitat preferences (Jenkins & Ormerod 2002). In the case of the bearded tit Panurus biarmicus, using playback showed that these birds occupied many other sites and habitats than previously thought without playback (Surmacki 2003).

Similarly, we found that the playback effect was affected by the presence of foxes, with actually no effect being detected when fox were present. Smaller numbers of grey partridges in sites with fox may be interpreted as avoidance of fox or as a consequence of the pressure of fox predation. Because fox activity is highest at dusk and at night (Cavallini 1996, Pandolfi et al. 1997), we cannot rule out that grey partridges, whose peak vocal activity occurs after sunset, may have been shunning sites near fox burrows or at least avoiding vocal activity at this time of day. Confirmation of this may be the smaller increase of calling male partridges after playback in sites with fox burrows compared to locations where such burrows were not present. Most grey partridge brood losses in western Poland are caused by fox (Panek 2005), and an experimental reduction of the number of predators (including fox) improved the breeding success and increased densities of the grey partridge (Tapper et al. 1996). The strong predation pressure of fox on the grey partridge is confirmed by the significant proportion of Galliformes species in its diet (Gołdyń et al. 2003, Baker et al. 2006). The negative impact of fox presence has also been shown on other ground nesting birds in open farmland (Tryjanowski et al. 2002). The results obtained in our study are also important in the context of determining whether changes in the numbers of grey partridge are based on habitat preferences or the intensification of farming practices (Panek & Kamieniarz 1998, Klansek 2002, Wübbenhorst & Leuschner 2006). The observed increase in fox numbers may possibly be an alternative cause of decreasing numbers of grey partridges.

Acknowledgements -

we would like to thank Cezary Mitrus and the anonymous reviewers for their critical comments on the manuscript.

References

1.

N.J. Aebischer & J.A. Ewald 2004: Managing the UK Grey Partridge Perdix perdix recovery: population change, reproduction, habitat and shooting. - Ibis 146: (Suppl. 2) 181–191. Google Scholar

2.

N.J. Aebischer & B. Kavanagh 1997: Grey Partridge. In: W.J.M. Hagemeijer & M.J. Blair (Eds); The EBCC Atlas of European breeding birds, their distribution and abundance.. T & AD Poyser, London, pp. 212–213. Google Scholar

3.

P. Baker , M. Furlong , S. Southern & S. Harris 2006: The potential impact of red fox Vulpes vulpes predation in agricultural landscape in lowland Britain. - Wildlife Biology 12: 39–50. Google Scholar

4.

J.C. Blanco 1986: On the diet, size and use of home range and activity patterns of a red fox in Central Spain. - Acta Theriologica 31: 547–556. Google Scholar

5.

M. Brambilla & D. Rubolini 2004: Water Rail Rallus aquaticus breeding density and habitat preferences in Northern Italy. - Ardea 92: 11–18. Google Scholar

6.

E. Bro , P. Mayot , E. Corda & F. Reitz 2004: Impact of habitat management on grey partridge populations: assessing wildlife cover using a multisite BACI experiment. - Journal of Applied Ecology 41: 846–857. Google Scholar

7.

P. Cavallini 1996: Ranging behaviour of red foxes during the mating and breeding seasons. - Ethology Ecology and Evolution 8: 57–65. Google Scholar

8.

C.J. Conway , W.R. Eddleman , S.H. Anderson & L.R. Henebury 1993: Seasonal changes in Yuma clapper rail vocalization rate and habitat use. - Journal of Wildlife Management 57: 282–290. Google Scholar

9.

S.A. Evans , F. Mougeot , S.M. Redpath & F. Leckie 2007: Alternative methods for estimating density in an upland game bird: the red grouse Lagopus lagopus scoticus. - Wildlife Biology 13: 130–139. Google Scholar

10.

A. Goławski & W. Meissner 2008: The influence of territory characteristics and food supply on the breeding performance of the Red-backed Shrike (Lanius collurio) in an extensively farmed region of eastern Poland. - Ecological Research 23: 347–353. Google Scholar

11.

B. Gołdyn , M. Hromada , A. Sarmacki & P. Tryjanowski 2003: Habitat use and diet of the red fox Vulpes vulpes in agricultural landscape in Poland. - Zeitschrift für Jagdwissenschaft 49: 191–200. Google Scholar

12.

S.L. Granholm 1983: Bias in density estimates due to movement of birds. - Condor 85: 243–248. Google Scholar

13.

B.A. Hahn & E.D. Silverman 2007: Managing breeding forest songbirds with conspecific song playbacks. - Animal Conservation 10: 436–441. Google Scholar

14.

R.K.B. Jenkins & S.J. Ormerod 2002: Habitat preferences of breeding Water Rails Rallus aquations. - Bird Study 49(1): 2–10. Google Scholar

15.

G.D. Kearns , N.B. Kwartin , D.F. Binker & G.M. Haramis 1998: Digital playback and improved trap design enhances capture of migrant soras and Virginia rails. - Journal of Field Ornithology 69: 466–473. Google Scholar

16.

E. Klansek 2002: Effects of landscape structure on Perdix perdix in Austria; outline and outlook. - Zeitschrift für Jagdwissenschaft 48: 340–345. Google Scholar

17.

M. Pandolfi , P. Forconi & L. Montecchiari 1997: Spatial behaviour of the red fox (Vulpes vulpes) in a rural area of central Italy. - Italian Journal of Zoology 64: 351–358. Google Scholar

18.

M. Panek 1992: Mechanisms determining population levels and density regulation in polish Grey Partridges (Perdix perdix). - Gibier Fauna Sauvage 9: 325–335. Google Scholar

19.

M. Panek 1994: The effect of land-use changes on populations of Partridge (Perdix perdix) in Poland. - In: L. Ryszkowski & S. Balazy (Eds); Functional Appraisal of Agricultural Landscape in Europe Research Center for Agricultural and Forest Environment, pp. 197–203. Google Scholar

20.

M. Panek 1998: Use of call counts for estimating spring density of the Grey partridge Perdix perdix. - Acta Ornithologica 33: 143–148. Google Scholar

21.

M. Panek 2005: Demography of grey partridges Perdix perdix in Poland in the years 1991–2004: reasons of population decline. - European Journal of Wildlife Research 51: 14–18. (DOI: 10.1007/s10344-005-0079-y). Google Scholar

22.

M. Panek & W. Bresiński 2002: Red fox Vulpes vulpes density and habitat use in a rural area of western Poland in the end of 1990s, compared with the turn of 1970s. - Acta Theriologica 47: 433–442. Google Scholar

23.

M. Panek & R. Kamieniarz 1998: Agricultural landscape structure and density of grey partridge (Perdix perdix) populations in Poland. - Gibier Faune Sauvage, Game Wildlife 15: 309–320. Google Scholar

24.

D.M.B. Parish & N.W. Sotherton 2007: The fate of released captive-reared grey partridges Perdix perdix: implications for reintroduction programmes. - Wildlife Biology 13: 140–149. Google Scholar

25.

D. Pepin & M. Birkan 1981: Comparative total - and strip-census estimates of hares and partridges. - Acta Oecologica 2: 151–160. Google Scholar

26.

T. Petrusková , A. Petrusek , V. Pavel & R. Fuchs 2007: Territorial meadow pipit males (Anthus pratensis; Passeriformes) become more aggressive in female presence. - Naturwissenschaften 94: 643–650. Google Scholar

27.

G.R. Potts 1986: The Partridge: Pesticides, Predation and Conservation. - Collins, London, 274 pp. Google Scholar

28.

J.C. Roché 1990: All the bird songs of Britain and Europe. - Sittelle Google Scholar

29.

J.J. Rotella & J.T. Ratti 1986: Test of a critical density index assumption: a case study with gray partridge. - Journal of Wildlife Management 50: 532–539. Google Scholar

30.

M. Šalek , P. Marhoul , J. Pintiř , T. Kopecký & L. Slabý 2004: Importance of unmanged wasteland patches for the grey partridge Perdix perdix in suburban habitats. - Acta Oecologica 25: 25–33. Google Scholar

31.

J. Schoppers 1996: Cassetterecorder goed hulpmiddel bij inventarisatie Patrijs Perdix perdix in het broedseizoen. (In Dutch with an English summary: Tape recorder useful in breeding season surveys of Partridge Perdix perdix). - Limosa 69: 180–181. Google Scholar

32.

R.R. Sokal & F.J. Rohlf 2001: Biometry. - W.H. Freeman and Company, New York, 887 pp. Google Scholar

33.

StatSoft, Inc. 2003: STATISTICA (data analysis software system), version 6. - Available at:  www.statsoft.comGoogle Scholar

34.

A. Surmacki 2003: Use of playback in estimating numbers of Bearded Tit Panurus biarmicus outside the breeding season. - Ornis Hungarica 12–13: 303–305. Google Scholar

35.

S.C. Tapper , G.R. Potts & M.H. Brockless 1996: The effect of an experimental reduction in predation pressure on the breeding success and population density of grey partridges Perdix perdix. - Journal of Applied Ecology 33: 965–978. Google Scholar

36.

P. Tryjanowski , B. Gołdyn & A. Surmacki 2002: Influence of the red fox (Vulpes vulpes, Linnaeus 1758) on the distribution and number of breeding birds in an intensively used farmland. - Ecological Research 17: 395–399. Google Scholar

37.

P. Tryjanowski , M. Hromada , M. Antczak , J. Grzybek , S. Kuźniak & G. Lorek 2003: Which method is most suitable for censusing breeding populations of redbacked (Lanius collurio) and great grey (Lanius excubitor) shrikes? - Omis Hungarica 12–13: 223–228. Google Scholar

38.

J. Verner 1985: Assessment of counting techniques. - Current Ornithology 2: 247–302. Google Scholar

39.

J.P. Wiegand 1980: Ecology of the Hungarian partridge in north-central Montana. - Wildlife Monographs 74: 1–106. Google Scholar

40.

D. Wübbenhorst & C. Leuschner 2006: Vegetation structure at the breeding sites of the partridge (Perdix predix L.) in Central Europe and its possible importance for population density. - Polish Journal of Ecology 54: 57–67. Google Scholar

41.

I. Zuberogoitia & L.F. Campos 1998: Censusing owls in large areas: A comparison between methods. - Ardeola 45: 47–53. Google Scholar
© Wildlife Biology, NKV
Zbigniew Kasprzykowski and Artur Goławski "Does the Use of Playback Affect the Estimates of Numbers of Grey Partridge Perdix perdix?," Wildlife Biology 15(2), 123-128, (1 June 2009). https://doi.org/10.2981/08-001
Received: 3 January 2008; Accepted: 4 August 2008; Published: 1 June 2009
KEYWORDS
density
Farmland
fox
grey partridge
Perdix perdix
playback
vocal activity
Back to Top