Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 May 2006 DEMOGRAPHY, GENETICS, AND THE VALUE OF MIXED MESSAGES
John M. Pearce, Sandra L. Talbot
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Iverson et al. (2004) used estimates of the homing rate for molting adult Harlequin Ducks (Histrionicus histrionicus) in Alaska to draw inferences about population structure. Homing rates, defined as one minus the ratio of birds recaptured elsewhere to those recaptured at the original banding site, were high (0.95–1.00) for males and females. Iverson et al. (2004) concluded that these high rates of homing are indicative of demographic independence among molting groups separated by small distances (tens to hundreds of kilometers) and that conservation efforts should recognize this fine-scale population structure. We re-examined their use of the homing rate, because their assumption of equal detection probability across a wide sampling area could have led to an upward bias in their estimates of site fidelity. As a result, we are hesitant to agree with their conclusion of high adult homing to molting areas and that molt-site fidelity is evidence for demographic independence. Our hesitancy stems from the fact that little is known about juvenile and adult movements within and among years, breeding area origins, and the variation of demographic parameters (e.g., survival and productivity) among molting groups. Furthermore, population genetic data of these molting groups suggest gene flow at both nuclear and mitochondrial loci. Such mixed messages between demographic (i.e., banding) and genetic data are increasingly common in ornithological studies and offer unique opportunities to reassess predictions and make more robust inferences about population structure across broad temporal and spatial scales. Thus, we stress that it is this broader scale perspective, which combines both demography and genetics, that biologists should seek to quantify and conservation efforts should seek to recognize.

Structured genetic and demographic patterns are predicted for species that exhibit high levels of natal philopatry (Avise 2004:499). Such patterns are not predicted when natal philopatry is low, even if adults are site faithful, because juvenile dispersal and subsequent reproduction will lead to gene flow among populations (Cabe 1999, Lovette et al. 2004, Arsenault et al. 2005). Yet several molecular examinations of avian species presumed to exhibit natal philopatry (Ransom et al. 2001, Roeder et al. 2001, Kimura et al. 2002, Pearce et al. 2004), or those documented to exhibit high levels of natal philopatry through banding studies (Avise et al. 1992, Birt-Friesen et al. 1992, Austin et al. 1994), have found sampled populations to be largely homogeneous at neutral genetic loci. These mixed messages arise for two general and nonmutually exclusive reasons: differences in the scale of measurement, and dispersal that results in gene flow.

First, demographic (i.e., banding) and genetic data assess patterns across very different geographic and temporal scales, thus mixed messages are not restricted to avian taxa (Tallmon et al. 2002, Hauswaldt and Glenn 2005). Genetic methods infer dispersal via gene flow over much larger geographic scales than is usually possible with banding data. Furthermore, no genetic estimates of dispersal are truly contemporary, but rather represent an average rate of gene flow across multiple generations up to some point in the recent past (Bossart and Prowell 1998). Thus, historical events, such as those related to post-Pleistocene ice movements (Hewitt 2000), can create a condition whereby insufficient time has elapsed since range expansions for natal philopatry to result in genetic differences among populations (Slatkin 1987, Avise 2004). Second, juvenile dispersal and subsequent reproduction results in gene flow among populations (Cabe 1999, Frederiksen et al. 2002, Arsenault et al. 2005). As a result, numerous authors have argued for a combined, multimarker approach to quantify historical and ongoing factors to best infer population patterns (Avise et al. 1992, Bossart and Prowell 1998, Koenig et al. 2000, Kimura et al. 2002, Kendall and Nichols 2004).

Mixed messages have recently confronted researchers investigating molting and wintering groups of Harlequin Ducks (Histrionicus histrionicus) in Alaska and British Columbia. Lanctot et al. (1999) used mitochondrial (mt) DNA control region sequence data and nuclear microsatellite loci to characterize molting groups of Harlequin Ducks in southcentral Alaska. Results suggested that sampling areas were largely homogeneous for both types of genetic markers, although slightly higher levels of differentiation were observed between more distant regions using mtDNA. This result was surprising as Cooke et al. (2000) observed adult Harlequin Ducks to exhibit high levels of site fidelity to molting and wintering areas of coastal British Columbia, which they suggested could lead to genetic differentiation of wintering aggregations if mate choice occurs in these locations. Following many years of intensive study, Iverson et al. (2004) were able to quantify between-year rates of adult molt-site fidelity of Harlequin Ducks in Prince William Sound, Alaska via the homing rate. They observed high homing rates (0.95–1.0) and concluded that these estimates are indicative of demographic independence among molting groups at a finer scale than indicated by genetic data.

Here, we re-examine the homing rate estimator used by Iverson et al. (2004), their conclusion that high male and female adult homing rates are indicative of demographic independence, and offer a review of the distinction between demographic and genetic population structure. One note on terminology: we feel the word ‘philopatry’ does not adequately apply to nonbreeding areas and that a species is philopatric only if dispersal is limited with respect to an organism's natal area (Greenwood 1980, Shields 1982). Therefore, because Harlequin Ducks in Prince William Sound do not breed where they molt, we use the term ‘adult molt-site fidelity’ instead of winter philopatry, since Iverson et al. (2004) were concerned with the return of adults to molting areas.

Demography

Iverson et al. (2004) captured and marked molting Harlequin Ducks throughout Prince William Sound, Alaska from 1995 to 1997 and 2000 to 2001. Adult males and females were marked with standard metal leg bands. Hatch-year birds were capable of flight and thus unavailable for capture and marking. Banding data were analyzed using an estimator of homing rate, which Iverson et al. (2004:712) define as “the ratio of birds recaptured on their original capture site to those recaptured elsewhere (Robertson and Cooke 1999).” This definition of homing rate differs from that given by Anderson et al. (1992), which includes an estimation of fidelity (F), temporary emigration (γ), and recapture (P) given presence. It also differs from the return rate (R), which includes an estimation of survival (S) as well as F, γ, and P (Williams et al. 2002). These estimators can be biased due to confounding among the various parameters, but the direction of the bias is known because noncapture probabilities are estimated. Thus, for the homing rate of Iverson et al. (2004) to accurately quantify fidelity, all marked birds must be recaptured with the same probability regardless of their subsequent molting location (i.e., P cannot vary between birds that return and those that disperse). In other words, the homing rate estimator

i0010-5422-108-2-474-e01.gif
where S drops out, cannot simplify to F unless P1  =  P2. Iverson et al. (2004) acknowledged this assumption and used multiple ‘supplementary sites’ to document any dispersal events away from the primary Montague Island sites. However, given the fact that most dispersal events detected by Iverson et al. (2004) were of short distances (3.1–8.9 km), our prediction is that dispersal events to supplementary sites >8.9 km should have a lower P than those within 8.9 km of the primary capture locations. Additionally, the longest dispersal event documented via recapture was 51.9 km. Therefore, all Harlequin Ducks within 51.9 km of their initial capture location must have the same probability of recapture following dispersal as those that do not disperse for the assumption of equal capture probability to be met. Thus, the assumption of equal P across Prince William Sound may not have been appropriate and a lower P for supplementary sites may have resulted in an overestimate of the homing rate at primary sites.

Knowledge of several aspects of movement ecology would enhance the characterization of molt-site fidelity in Harlequin Ducks, such as the distribution of molting birds along Montague Island. Do unsampled molting flocks that could contain marked birds occur close to the three primary sampling areas? What is the number of birds banded at time t, not observed at time t + 1, but subsequently observed at time t + i? Such data would provide information on detection probabilities, movements among molting sites, and the timing of molt migrations. Also, what is known about the timing of molt for nonbreeding versus brood-rearing females? Are nonbreeding and successfully breeding females equally available for capture during molt drives? Cooke et al. (2000) observed that both younger and older males move among areas and that winter site fidelity may be contingent upon the availability of unpaired females or territoriality of paired males. Thus, information on the number, sex, and age of unbanded birds captured by Iverson et al. (2004) is of interest. Such information seems necessary to fully evaluate the conclusion of Iverson et al. (2004) that molting flocks exhibit high site fidelity and are demographically independent. Demographic independence implies that each molting group is characterized by unique rates of survival and reproduction. If solely males move among molting groups, there is the possibility that population structure exists among females, but nothing is known about the location of breeding areas of female Harlequin Ducks that molt and winter throughout Prince William Sound. Furthermore, low levels of differentiation among females for maternally inherited mtDNA (see below) suggest that movement among molting areas is not strictly limited to males. In the absence of such information, it is unclear how adult male and female homing rates are indicative of unique demographic and thus conservation units at such a small scale.

Genetics

Behavioral traits of waterfowl have led to various hypotheses about the location and degree of population genetic structure. First, in most waterfowl species females exhibit greater natal and breeding site fidelity than males (Greenwood 1980, Anderson et al. 1992). Over sufficient periods of time and spatial scale, natal philopatry can lead to genetic differentiation, especially at maternally inherited molecular loci such as mtDNA. Second, both sexes may exhibit winter site fidelity for a variety of ecological and genetic reasons, including re-establishment of breeding pairs as mate choice is thought to occur on the wintering grounds (reviewed by Robertson and Cooke 1999). In either case adult nest or winter site fidelity might also serve to differentiate populations, but only at larger scales where populations are separated at distances greater than all dispersal events. However, even at these larger scales, genetic differentiation can be limited by historical demographic processes and large effective population sizes, which reduces the diversifying forces of mutation and drift (reviewed by Avise 2004).

In Harlequin Ducks, molt-site fidelity would only lead to demographic and population genetic structure if molting flocks remain as distinct units throughout the entire year. Regehr et al. (2001) observed that Harlequin Duck broods may accompany adult females to molting sites. However, it is not known if juvenile Harlequin Ducks, especially females, return to these same molting areas as adults in subsequent years. Tracking natal dispersal of birds via mark-recapture methods is very difficult because efforts must be made to recapture previously marked individuals across a wide range of possible dispersal distances. This difficulty is compounded among sea duck species, which typically do not breed until their second or third year. Thus, data from multiple years and wide geographical efforts would be needed to discern the range of possible dispersal events (Lebreton et. al. 2003). However, if dispersal is male-biased, one might predict higher levels of population differentiation using maternally inherited mtDNA in contrast to nuclear DNA, which tracks both maternal and paternal lineages. Lanctot et al. (1999) observed precisely this pattern, with slightly elevated levels of differentiation for mtDNA in comparison to nuclear microsatellite data among regional groupings of female Harlequin Ducks. Still, the level of mtDNA differentiation observed by Lanctot et al. (1999) was low (ΦST  =  0.05) suggesting some female gene flow, but at a reduced level compared to males.

Cooke et al. (2000) viewed the genetic data of Lanctot et al. (1999) as validation of their conclusions of male-mediated gene flow, but Iverson et al. (2004) found the genetic data unable to “indicate” the fine-scale population structure suggested by the homing rate. First, little is known about the breeding area origins of molting Harlequin Ducks in Prince William Sound. Genetic data would only indicate fine-scale structure if each molting flock remained isolated throughout the year and over thousands of generations. A more likely scenario is that each molting flock is composed of birds from multiple breeding areas. Second, consider the genetic implications of demographic independence on such a fine scale. If juveniles follow females to molting sites each year and then both sexes continually return to those sites each year at a homing rate of 0.95–1.0 (Iverson et al. 2004), two immediate predictions for these flocks are: (1) elevated levels of inbreeding within each “demographically independent” molting flock, and (2) substantially higher levels of mtDNA population differentiation due to the smaller effective population size of mtDNA compared to nuclear loci and the small size of each molting flock (n 30), compounding genetic drift. Neither of these predictions is borne out by Lanctot et al. (1999). Genotype data from five microsatellite loci showed no significant heterozygote deficiencies, inbreeding coefficients did not differ significantly from zero, and levels of population differentiation estimated using mtDNA were low.

Other hypotheses concerning the population genetics of wintering Harlequin Ducks in Prince William Sound include: (1) a lack of power among genetic markers to distinguish wintering groups, (2) insufficient time since populations expanded into northern latitudes for site fidelity patterns to accrue genetic differences, and (3) molt-site fidelity is a complex behavioral process. A lack of power seems unlikely because fewer genetic loci have shown population differentiation in other waterfowl that exhibit high levels of adult site fidelity (Pearce et al. 2000). Harlequin Ducks have likely experienced historical population growth and expansion since the last glacial maximum. Consistently low levels of nucleotide diversity (near zero) across populations, as observed by Lanctot et al. (1999), are indicative of sudden population growth and expected if time is sufficient for recovery of haplotype variation via mutation, but too short for the accumulation of differences among sequences (reviewed by Avise 2000). Lastly, Lanctot et al. (1999) correctly acknowledged that movements of subadult birds among molting sites before patterns of adult fidelity are established might nullify the effects of high adult fidelity to these same sites (as observed in other species; Cabe 1999, Arsenault et al. 2005). No increase in DNA samples or genetic loci would recover a signal of population differentiation if natal and adult dispersal occur among these molting groups. Such movements would also erode any demographic independence among these same groups.

The hypothesis of molt or winter site fidelity leading to demographic and genetic structure deserves further examination because anthropogenic effects on sea duck populations often occur on the wintering grounds (Flint et al. 1999, Esler et al. 2000, Camphuysen et al. 2002). However, recent work suggests little genetic evidence for population structure among wintering sea ducks at either regional (Lanctot et al. 1999) or continental scales (Pearce et al. 2004). Additionally, the literature presented by Iverson et al. (2004) does not support a hypothesis that strong affiliations to nonbreeding areas are common among sea duck species. Papers by Alison (1974) and Savard (1985) are each based upon a single marked bird, and Limpert (1980) estimated a homing rate of 39%. Of 26 male and two female Harlequin Ducks banded by Breault and Savard (1999), nine males were seen at the same location in the following year, while two males were seen at an adjacent molting site. Thus, this and other literature (Cooke et al. 2000, Flint et al. 2000, Hatton and Marquiss 2004, Mehl et al. 2004) suggests that annual affiliations to molting areas by sea ducks are quite variable. Lastly, we view molt-site fidelity as a less than robust measure of population structure without evidence that molting flocks originate from distinct breeding areas. Under the scenario that molting flocks are composed of birds from a variety of breeding areas, fidelity is instead an intriguing behavioral pattern and not a measure of demographic independence.

The Value of Mixed Messages

Conservation plans often seek to delineate geographic or taxonomic units as distinct population segments to effectively monitor status and trends (U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service and National Marine Fisheries Service 1996). Defining such units relies upon a wide array of criteria that for migratory birds may include morphological or plumage characteristics, demographic patterns quantified by banding or radio-telemetry, molecular genetic data, or geographic separation of population segments during the annual cycle. However, focusing on only one type of data to define such units misdirects valuable research and conservation efforts (Zink et al. 2000) and tends to promote population structure as a binary condition: populations are either structured or they are not (Crandall et al. 2000). A larger set of direct and indirect markers are now available for assessing movement patterns (reviewed by Webster et al. 2002, Kendall and Nichols 2004), as well as novel analytical methods that estimate levels, directionality, and sources of variation in dispersal rather than simply testing dispersal as a binary condition (Pritchard et al. 2000, Hey and Nielsen 2004, Kendall and Nichols 2004). Thus, we wish to stress that behavioral patterns among avian populations are intricate and idiosyncratic (Avise et al. 1992, Zink et al. 2003, Coltman 2004), and efforts should focus on the robust quantification of these multifaceted processes with as much information as possible.

For example, a number of avian genetic studies discuss the impact of historical demographic processes on levels of population differentiation and the need for other, nongenetic data to verify or challenge conclusions (Bossart and Prowell 1998, Kimura et al. 2002, Pearce et al. 2005). Similarly, banding studies have called for DNA-based estimates of movement after recognizing the difficulty of detecting and quantifying long-distance dispersal (Koenig et al. 2000, Arsenault et al. 2005). In a joint analysis of banding and genetic data for the Lesser Snow Goose (Anser caerulescens), Avise et al. (1992:1094) argued appropriately that “both evolutionary (genetic) and contemporary (behavioral) perspectives are required for a full appreciation of the geographic population structure of a species.”

An examination of the table of contents of recent ornithological and molecular ecology journals reveals the current focus on demographic parameters (e.g., nesting success, productivity, survival, dispersal, and colonization history) and their role in population status and trends. Yet a common misconception is that genetic data are a panacea for inferring population structure. Instead, molecular genetic markers should be viewed as offering a singular but multifaceted perspective on population differentiation and demography. Regardless of whether genetic or nongenetic methods are used, we encourage researchers to use multiple data types when they are available. Even in cases where genetic data suggest no differentiation among sampling locales, such as among molting groups of Harlequin Ducks, there is still a wealth of information that can be inferred from the molecular information, such as historical population trends (Emerson et al. 2001), geographic variation in genetic diversity (Busch et al. 2000, Zink et al. 2000), relative levels of female natal philopatry and male dispersal (Pearce et al. 2005), or evidence for gene flow via dispersal that is difficult to assess with localized banding data (Arsenault et al. 2005). Because inferences from multiple markers reveal the weaknesses and strengths of each method, comparisons among data types provide a competing model framework to reassess predictions, evaluate temporal and spatial scales, and best infer population patterns.

Acknowledgments

We thank D. Esler and S. Iverson for the numerous amicable and engaging discussions on topics presented here. C. Ely, D. Derksen, P. Flint, M. Lindberg, K. McCracken, J. Schmidt, D. Dobkin, T. Sachtleben, and one anonymous reviewer offered comments on earlier drafts of the paper.

Literature Cited

1.

R. M. Alison 1974. Oldsquaw homing in winter. Auk 91:188. Google Scholar

2.

M. G. Anderson, J. M. Rhymer, and F. C. Rowher . 1992. Philopatry, dispersal, and the genetic structure of waterfowl populations,. 365–395. in B.D. Batt, A.D. Afton, M.G. Anderson, C.D. Ankney, D.H. Johnson, J.A. Kadlec, G.L. Krapu [eds.],. Ecology and management of breeding waterfowl University of Minnesota Press Minneapolis, MN. Google Scholar

3.

D. P. Arsenault, P. B. Stacey, and G. A. Hoelzer . 2005. Mark-recapture and DNA fingerprinting data reveal high breeding-site fidelity, low natal philopatry, and low levels of genetic population differentiation in Flammulated Owls (Otus flammeolus). Auk 122:329–337. Google Scholar

4.

J. J. Austin, R. W. White, and J. R. Ovenden . 1994. Population-genetic structure of a philopatric, colonially nesting seabird, the Short-tailed Shearwater (Puffinus tenuirostris). Auk 111:70–79. Google Scholar

5.

J. C. Avise 2000. Phylogeography: the history and formation of species. Harvard University Press Cambridge, MA. Google Scholar

6.

J. C. Avise 2004. Molecular markers, natural history and evolution. 2nd ed. Sinauer Sunderland, MA. Google Scholar

7.

J. C. Avise, R. T. Alisauskas, W. S. Nelson, and C. D. Ankney . 1992. Matriarchal population genetic structure in an avian species with female natal philopatry. Evolution 46:1804–1096. Google Scholar

8.

V. L. Birt-Friesen, W. A. Montevecchi, A. J. Gaston, and W. S. Davidson . 1992. Genetic structure of Thick-billed Murre (Uria lomvia) populations examined using direct sequence analysis of amplified DNA. Evolution 46:267–272. Google Scholar

9.

J. L. Bossart and D. P. Prowell . 1998. Genetic estimates of population structure and gene flow: limitations, lessons and new directions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13:202–206. Google Scholar

10.

A. M. Breault and J-P. L. Savard . 1999. Philopatry of Harlequin Ducks moulting in southern British Columbia,. 41–44. in R.I. Goudie, M.R. Petersen, G.J. Robertson [eds.],. Behaviour and ecology of sea ducks Canadian Wildlife Service Occasional Paper No. 100 Ottawa, ON, Canada. Google Scholar

11.

J. D. Busch, M. P. Miller, E. H. Paxton, M. K. Sogge, and P. Keim . 2000. Genetic variation in the endangered southwestern Willow Flycatcher. Auk 117:586–595. Google Scholar

12.

P. R. Cabe 1999. Dispersal and population structure in the European Starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Condor 101:451–454. Google Scholar

13.

C. J. Camphuysen, C. M. Berrevoets, H. J. Cremers, A. Dekinga, R. Dekker, B. J. Ens, T. M. van der Have, R. K. Kats, T. Kuiken, and M. F. Leopold . 2002. Mass mortality of Common Eiders (Somateria mollissima) in the Dutch Wadden Sea, winter 1999/2000: starvation in a commercially exploited wetland of international importance. Biological Conservation 106:303–317. Google Scholar

14.

D. W. Coltman 2004. Differentiation by dispersal. Nature 433:23–24. Google Scholar

15.

F. Cooke, G. J. Robertson, C. M. Smith, R. I. Goudie, and W. S. Boyd . 2000. Survival, emigration, and winter population structure of Harlequin Ducks. Condor 102:137–144. Google Scholar

16.

K. A. Crandall, O. R P. Bininda-Emonds, G. M. Mace, and R. K. Wayne . 2000. Considering evolutionary processes in conservation biology. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 15:290–295. Google Scholar

17.

B. C. Emerson, E. Paradis, and C. Thebaud . 2001. Revealing the demographic histories of species using DNA sequences. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 16:707–716. Google Scholar

18.

D. Esler, J. A. Schmutz, R. L. Jarvis, and D. M. Mulcahy . 2000. Winter survival of adult female Harlequin Ducks in relation to history of contamination by the Exxon Valdez oil spill. Journal of Wildlife Management 64:839–847. Google Scholar

19.

P. L. Flint, A. C. Fowler, and R. F. Rockwell . 1999. Modeling bird mortality associated with the M/V Citrus oil spill off St. Paul Island, Alaska. Ecological Modelling 117:261–267. Google Scholar

20.

P. L. Flint, M. R. Petersen, C. P. Dau, J. E. Hines, and J. D. Nichols . 2000. Annual survival and site fidelity of Steller's Eiders molting along the Alaska Peninsula. Journal of Wildlife Management 64:261–268. Google Scholar

21.

M. Fredericksen, T. Bregnballe, M. R. van Eerden, S. van Rijn, and J. D. Lebreton . 2002. Site fidelity of wintering cormorants Phalacrocorax carbo sinensis in Europe. Wildlife Biology 8:241–250. Google Scholar

22.

J. Greenwood 1980. Mating systems, philopatry and dispersal in birds and mammals. Animal Behaviour 28:1140–1162. Google Scholar

23.

P. L. Hatton and M. Marquiss . 2004. The origins of molting Goosanders on the Eden Estuary. Ringing and Migration 22:70–74. Google Scholar

24.

J. S. Hauswaldt and T. C. Glenn . 2005. Population genetics of the diamondback terrapin (Malaclemys terrapin). Molecular Ecology 14:723–732. Google Scholar

25.

G. Hewitt 2000. The genetic legacy of the Quaternary ice ages. Nature 405:907–913. Google Scholar

26.

J. Hey and R. Nielsen . 2004. Multilocus methods for estimating population sizes, migration rates and divergence time, with applications to the divergence of Drosophila pseudoobscura and D. persimilis. Genetics 167:747–760. Google Scholar

27.

S. A. Iverson, D. Esler, and D. J. Rizzolo . 2004. Winter philopatry of Harlequin Ducks in Prince William Sound, Alaska. Condor 106:711–715. Google Scholar

28.

W. L. Kendall and J. D. Nichols . 2004. On the estimation of dispersal and movement of birds. Condor 106:720–731. Google Scholar

29.

M. Kimura, S. M. Clegg, I. J. Lovette, K. R. Holder, D. J. Girman, B. Mila, P. Wade, and T. B. Smith . 2002. Phylogeographical approaches to assessing demographic connectivity between breeding and overwintering regions in a Nearctic-Neotropical warbler (Wilsonia pusilla). Molecular Ecology 11:1605–1616. Google Scholar

30.

W. D. Koenig, P. N. Hooge, M. T. Stanback, and J. Haydock . 2000. Natal dispersal in the cooperatively breeding Acorn Woodpecker. Condor 102:492–502. Google Scholar

31.

R. Lanctot, B. Goatcher, K. T. Scribner, S. L. Talbot, B. Pierson, D. Esler, and D. Zwiefelhofer . 1999. Harlequin Duck recovery from the Exxon Valdez oil spill: a population genetics perspective. Auk 116:781–791. Google Scholar

32.

J-D. Lebreton, J. E. Hines, R. Pradel, J. D. Nichols, and J. A. Spendelow . 2003. Estimation by capture-recapture of recruitment and dispersal over several sites. Oikos 101:253–264. Google Scholar

33.

R. J. Limpert 1980. Homing success of adult Buffleheads to a Maryland wintering site. Journal of Wildlife Management 44:905–908. Google Scholar

34.

I. J. Lovette, S. M. Clegg, and T. B. Smith . 2004. Limited utility of mtDNA markers for determining connectivity among breeding and overwintering locations in three Neotropical migrant birds. Conservation Biology 18:156–166. Google Scholar

35.

K. R. Mehl, R. T. Alisauskas, K. A. Hobson, and D. K. Kellett . 2004. To winter east or west? Heterogeneity in winter philopatry in a central-arctic population of King Eiders. Condor 106:241–251. Google Scholar

36.

J. M. Pearce, B. J. Pierson, S. L. Talbot, D. V. Derksen, D. Kraege, and K. T. Scribner . 2000. A genetic evaluation of morphology used to identify harvested Canada Geese. Journal of Wildlife Management 64:863–874. Google Scholar

37.

J. M. Pearce, S. L. Talbot, M. R. Petersen, and J. R. Rearick . 2005. Limited genetic differentiation among breeding, molting, and wintering groups of the threatened Steller's Eider: the role of historic and contemporary factors. Conservation Genetics 6 743–757. Google Scholar

38.

J. M. Pearce, S. L. Talbot, B. J. Pierson, K. T. Scribner, D. L. Dickson, and A. Mosbech . 2004. Lack of spatial genetic structure among nesting and wintering King Eiders. Condor 106:229–240. Google Scholar

39.

J. K. Pritchard, M. Stephens, and P. J. Donnelly . 2000. Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data. Genetics 155:945–959. Google Scholar

40.

D. Ransom Jr, R. L. Honeycutt, and R. D. Slack . 2001. Population genetics of southeastern Wood Ducks. Journal of Wildlife Management 65:745–754. Google Scholar

41.

H. M. Regehr, C. M. Smith, B. Arquilla, and F. Cooke . 2001. Post-fledging broods of migratory Harlequin Ducks accompany females to wintering areas. Condor 103:408–412. Google Scholar

42.

G. J. Robertson and F. Cooke . 1999. Winter philopatry in waterfowl. Auk 116:20–34. Google Scholar

43.

A. D. Roeder, R. K. Marshall, A. J. Mitchelson, T. Visagathilagar, P. A. Ritchie, D. R. Love, T. J. Pakai, H. C. McPartlan, N. D. Murray, N. A. Robinson, K. R. Kerry, and D. M. Lambert . 2001. Gene flow on the ice: genetic differentiation among Adelie Penguin colonies around Antarctica. Molecular Ecology 10:1645–1656. Google Scholar

44.

J-P. L. Savard 1985. Evidence of long-term pair bonds in Barrow's Goldeneye Bucephala islandica. Auk 102:389–391. Google Scholar

45.

W. M. Shields 1982. Philopatry, inbreeding, and the evolution of sex. State University of New York Press Albany, NY. Google Scholar

46.

M. Slatkin 1987. Gene flow and the geographic structure of natural populations. Science 236:787–792. Google Scholar

47.

D. A. Tallmon, H. M. Draheim, S. L. Mills, and F. W. Allendorf . 2002. Insights into recently fragmented vole populations from combined genetic and demographic data. Molecular Ecology 11:699–709. Google Scholar

48.

U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service and National Marine Fisheries Service 1996. Policy regarding the recognition of distinct vertebrate population segments under the endangered species act. Federal Register 61:4721–4725. Google Scholar

49.

M. S. Webster, P. P. Marra, S. M. Haig, S. Bensch, and R. T. Holmes . 2002. Links between worlds: unraveling migratory connectivity. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 17:76–83. Google Scholar

50.

B. K. Williams, J. D. Nichols, and M. J. Conroy . 2002. Estimating survival, movement, and other state transitions with mark-recapture methods,. 417–493. in B.K. Williams, J.D. Nichols, M.J. Conroy [eds.],. Analysis and management of animal populations Academic Press San Diego, CA. Google Scholar

51.

R. M. Zink, G. F. Barrowclough, J. L. Atwood, and R. C. Blackwell-Rago . 2000. Genetics, taxonomy, and conservation of the threatened California Gnatcatcher. Conservation Biology 14:1394–1405. Google Scholar

52.

R. M. Zink, S. V. Drovetski, S. Questau, I. V. Fadeev, E. V. Nesterov, M. C. Westberg, and S. Rohwer . 2003. Recent evolutionary history of the Bluethroat (Luscinia svecica) across Eurasia. Molecular Ecology 12:3069–3075. Google Scholar

Appendices

John M. Pearce and Sandra L. Talbot "DEMOGRAPHY, GENETICS, AND THE VALUE OF MIXED MESSAGES," The Condor 108(2), 474-479, (1 May 2006). https://doi.org/10.1650/0010-5422(2006)108[474:DGATVO]2.0.CO;2
Received: 24 May 2005; Accepted: 1 October 2005; Published: 1 May 2006
KEYWORDS
Harlequin Duck
Histrionicus histrionicus
homing rate
population structure
winter philopatry
Back to Top